Skip to main content

Full text of "ERIC ED285206: The Role of Task Representation in Reading-to-Write. Reading-to-Write: Exploring a Cognitive and Social Process Series, Report 2."

See other formats


DOCUMENT RESUME 



ED 285 206 



CS 210 789 



AUTHOR 
TITLE 



INSTITUTION 
SPONS AGENCY 

REPORT NO 
PUB DATE 
NOTE 

AVAILABLE FROM 



PUB TYPE 

EDRS PRICE 
DESCRIPTORS 



IDENTIFIERS 



ABSTRACT 



Flower, Linda 

The Role of Task Representation in Reading-to-Write . 
Reading-to-Write: Exploring a Cognitive and Social 
Process Series, Report 2. 

Center for the Study of Writing, Berkeley, CA. 

Office of Educational Research and Improvement (ED), 

Washington , DC. 

CSW-TR-6 

Jun 87 

41p. 

Center for the Study of Writing, School of Education, 
University of California, Berkeley CA 92720 ($3.00, 
plus sales tax for California residents; make checks 
payable to Regents of U.C.). 
Reports - Research/Technical (143) 

MF01/PC02 Plus Postage. 

College English; ^Content Area Writing; Expository 
Writing; Higher Education; Metacognition; Models; 
Prewriting; ^Problem Solving; ^Protocol Analysis; 
Reading Writing Relationship; *Task Analysis; 
♦Writing Instruction; ^Writing Processes; Writing 
Research; Writing Skills 

Response Based Writing; ^Writing Strategies 



Second in the series "Reading-to-Write : Exploring a 
Cognitive and Social Process," this report looks at the different 
ways students represent reading-to-write tasks to themselves, 
analyzes the resulting divergence in their writing goals and 
strategies, and recommends teaching task representation as an 
interpretive process that continues throughout composing. The report 
describes a task-representation study in which undergraduate and 
graduate English majors performed an open-ended reading-to-write task 
and transcribed their thinking-aloud protocols. Following the 
introduction and description of the study, the report discusses the 
various organizing plans elicited in the study, including plans to 
summarize, respond, review and comment, synthesize ideas around a 
controlling concept, and interpret or use ideas for a rhetorical 
purpose. The report then proposes the theory of task representation 
as a constructive process organized around the following principles: 
(1) the constructed task is an integration of a set of options and 
schemas analogous to menu choices offered on personal computer; (2) 
because task representation depends on noticing cues from the context 
and evoking r< levant memories, it can extend over the course of 
composing; anc (3) developments and changes in a writer's 
representation can lead to problems in constructing an integrated 
task and text. Finally, the report considers ways to evaluate 
different organizing plans and text types, arguing that the best plan 
is the one that fits both the situation and the writer's goals. 
(JG) 



9 

ERIC 



Center 

for 

the 
Study 

of 
Writing 



US DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION 

OHtce of Educational Research and Improvement 

EOUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION 
CENTER (ERIC) 

"•A This document has been reproduced as 
(received from the person or organisation 

originating it 
C Minor changes have been made to .mprove 
reproduction Quality 

• Pomts Of view or opinions stated m this docu 
ment do not necessarily represent official 
OERi position or policy 



University of California, Berkeley 
Carnegie Mellon University 



BEST COPY AVAILABLE 



CENTER FOR THE STUDY OF WRITING 



Technical Report No. 6 

THE ROLE OF TASK REPRESENTATION 
IN READING-TO-WRITE 

Linda Flower 

June, 1987 



This paper is Report 2 in the series entitled 
Readina-tQ-Write: Exploring a Punitiv e and Social Process 
by Linda Flower, John Ackerman, Margaret Kantz, Kathleen McCorrrnck, Wayne C. Peck, and 

Victoria Stein 



University of California Carnegie Mellon University 

Berkeley, CA 94720 Pittsburgh, PA 15213 



The project presented, or reported herein, was performed pursuant to a grant from the Office of 
Educational Research and Improvement/Department of Education (OERI/ED) for the Center for the 
Study of Writing. However, the opinions expressed herein do not necessarily reflect the position or 
policy of the OERI/ED and no official endorsement by the OERI/ED should be inferred. 



9 

ERIC 



\ 

\ 

READING-TO- WRITE: 
EXPLORING A COGNITIVE AND SOCIAL PROCESS 

Linda Flower, John Ackerman, Margaret Kantz, 
Kathleen McCormick, Wayne Peck, Victoria Stein 
Carnegie Mellon 

In an effort to understand reading-to- write as both a cognitive and a social process, this study was 
designed as a collaborative research project in which we tried to approach a common body of data 
from a number of different perspectives. The following reports arc part of this joint project from 
the Center for the Study of Writing at Carnegie Mellon, 

Report 1. Studying Cognition in Context: Introduction to the Study* 
Linda Flower 

Reading-to- write is an act of critical literacy central to much of academic 
discourse. This project, divided into an Exploratory Study and a Teaching Study, examines the 
cognitive processes of reading-to-write as they are embedded in the social context of a college 
course. 

Report 2. The Role of Task Representation in Reading-to- Write. 
Linda Flower 

The different ways in which students represented a "standard" 
reading-to-write task to themselves lead to markedly different goals and strategies as well as 
different organizing plans. This raised questions about the costs and benefits of such alternative 
representations for students and about students' metacognitive control of their own reading and 
writing processes. 

Report 3. Promises of Coherence, Weak Content, and Strong 
Organization: An Analysis of the Student Texts. 
Margaret Kantz 

Analysis of students 1 Organizing Plans (including free response, summary, 
review and comment, sj thesis, and interpretation for a rhetorical purpose) also revealed a hybrid 
plan in which certain coherence conventions gave the promise of synthesis while the papers 
substance reflected a simpler review and comment strategy* Both students and teachers, it 
appeared, may sometimes confuse coherence strategies (for text) with knowledge transformation 
strategies (for content). 

Report 4. Students' Self-Analyses and Judges 1 Perceptions: 
Where Do They Agree? John Ackerman 

Any writing assignment is a negotiation between a teachers expectations and 
a students representation of the task. Students 1 Self-Analysis Checklist showed a strong shift in 
perception for students in the experimental training condition, but a tellingly low (37%) agreement 
with judges 1 perceptions of the texts. 

Report 5. Exploring the Cognition of Reading-to-Write» 
Victoria Stein* 

A comparison of 17 freshmen and 19 older students revealed differences in 
ways students monitored their comprehension, structured the reading and planned their text. A 
study of these patterns of cognition and case studies of selected students revealed both seme 
successful and some problematic strategies students brought to their reading-to-write task. 



4 



Report 6. Elaboration: Using What You Know. Victoria Stein 



The process of elaboration allowed students to use prior knowledge not only 
for comprehension and critical thinking, but also structuring and planning their papers. However, 
surprising little of this valuable thinking found its way into students' papers. 

Report 7. The Effects of Prompts Upon Revision: A Glimpse 
of the Gap between Planning and Performance. 
Wayne C. Peck 

Students who were introduced to the options of task representation and 
prompted to attempt the difficult task of "interpruing for a purpose of one's own" on revision were 
far more likely to change their organizing plan than students prompted merely to revise to "make 
the text better." However, the protocols also revealed a significant group of students we called 
"Intenders" who, for various reasons, made plans they were unable to translate into text. 

Report 8. Translating Context into Action. John Ackerman 

One context for writing is the students history of schooling and high school 
assignments and essays. Based on protocols, texts, and interviews, this report descrioed a set of 
"initial reading strategies" nearly every freshman used to begin the task, strategies that appear to 
reflect their training in summarization and recitation of information. From this limited and often 
unexamined starting point, students then had to construct a solution path which either clung to, 
modified, or rejected this arhetorical initial approach to reading and writing. 

Report 9. The Cultural Imperatives Underlying Cognitive Acts. 
Kathleen McCormick 

By setting reading-to- write in a broad cultural context we explore some of the 
cultural imperatives that might underlie particular cognitive acts. Protocols and interviews suggest 
that three culturally-based assumptions played a role in this task: the desire for closure, a belief in 
objectivity, and a refusal to write about perceived contradictions. Alternative kinds of assignments 
can encourage both cognitive and cultural self-awareness. 

Report 10. The Transition to Academic Discourse. 
Linda Flower 

The process of comprehension provides a solid foundation for 
reading-to- write, so strong, in fact, that it can carry out most of the work in less complex versions 
of the task. As students moved from a comprehension-based process to more fully rhetorica!, 
purpose-guided composing process, they embedded strategies such as "gist and list" and the "true, 
important, I agree" strategy for selecting information into dialogues with the text and, in seme 
cases, into a constructive planning strategy focused on the problem of text-making itself. 

Report 11. Expanding the Repertoire: An Anthology of 

Practical Approaches for the Teaching of Writing. 
Kathleen McCormick et al. 

One important implication of this study is that students themselves should 
come into the act of examining their own reading and writing processes and becoming more aware 
of cognitive and cultural implications of their choices. This set of approaches, written by teachers 
collaborating on a Reading-to- Write course that grew out of this project, introduces students to 
ways of exploring their assumptions and alternative ways of representing aspects of the task. 



CENTER FOR THE STUDY OF WRITING 



Director Sarah Warshauer Freedrnan 

University of California, Berkeley 

Co-Directors Unda Flower J.R. Hayes 

Carnegie Mellon University Carnegie Mellon University 

James Gray 

University of California, Berkeley 

Administrative Director Sandra Schecter 

University of California, Berkeley 

Editor Melanie Sperling 

University of California, Berkeley 



Publication Review Board 

Chair Melanie Sperling 

University of California, Berkeley 

Assistant Chairs Charles Elster Karen Schriver 

University of California, Berkeley Carnegie Mellon University 

Advisors Charles Fillmore jj|| H. Larkin 

University of California, Berkeley Carnegie Mellon University 



Carta Asher, Herbert H Lehman College of the City University of New York 

Nancie Atwell, Boothbay Region Elementary School, Boothbay Harbor, ME 

Robert de Beaugrande, University of Florida 

Ruby Bernstein, Northgate High School, Walnut Creek, CA 

Wayne Booth, University of Chicago 

Robert CaJfee, Stanford University 

Michael Cole, University of California, San Diego 

Colette Daiute, Harvard University 

John Daly, University of Texas, Austin 

Peter Ebow, State University of New York, Stony Brc )k 

JoAnne T. Eresh, Writing and Speaking Center, Pittsburgh, PA 

Donald Graves, University of New Hampshire 

James Hahn, Fairfield High School, Fairfield, CA 

Julie Jensen, Unlvt • of Texas, Austin 

Andrea Lunsford, O. . - te University 

Marion M. Mohr, Fairfax unty Public Schools, Fairfax County, VA 

Lee Odell, Rensselaer Po^ .echnk Institute 

Charles Read, University of Wisconsin 

Victor Rentel, Ohio State Universe 

Michael W. Stubbs, University of London 

Deborah Tannen, Georgetown University 

Gordon Wells, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education 



THE ROLE OF TASK REPRESENTATION 
IN READING-TO'WRITE 



By 

Linda Rower 
Carnegie Mellon University 

Academic papers are typically written in the context of a rich rhetorical situation which 
includes not only the conventions of academic discourse, but the expectations of the instructor, the 
context of the course, and the terms of the assignment These requirements can seem so 
self-evident we are surprised when once again twenty students in a class interpret the same 
"standard" college writing assignment in strikingly different ways. This paper is about that act of 
inteipretation. Task representation is an interpretive process which translates the rhetorical 
situation-as the writer reads it-into the act of comoosing. As such, it is the major bridge which 
links the public context of writing with the private process of an individual writer. Therefore, let me 
introduce this process by sketching three public contexts in wh : ch students 1 task representations 
mattered. 

In the first, the instructors of the freshman composition course at my school were in our 
weekly seminar meeting, trying to understand/diagnose some of the student strategies behind the 
papers we were reading on die problem analysis assignment Writing a problem analysis, as we 
saw the task, was an occasion to struggle with a significant issue-a problem of the sort which 
resisted pat answers and called for the extra scrutiny writing allows. In the paper at hand, a 
young woman had written a polished, coherent essay on the problem posed by a "rainy day " with 
its awful train of decisions about choosing the right clothes and the dilemma of skipping puddles to 
class. A mildly clever, discouraging paper. It wasn't the sort of analysis we had in mmd. In the 
freshman literature course that same week the in$;ructors had spent a class session talking with 
students about how the response statements due Monday would allow, would even demand that 
students go beyond the summaries they had written in high school. A response statement, the 
instructors had discussed, asked students to record, then examine their own response. The first 
papers were in with students claiming they had indeed done this analysis. Fifty per cent were plot 
summaries. Meanwhile, over the bridge at the University of Pittsburgh was a thiid context which 
we can reconstruct from David Bartholomae's discussion of a student's freshman placement essay 
(1985). The reader on the placement committee had come to that place in the essay that would 
make or break it as acceptable academic discourse-tha would place this student in or out of basic 
writing: "At this point the [student] writer is in a perfect postion to speculate, to move from the 

problem to an analysis of the problem, however We get neither a technical discussion nor an 

'academic 1 discussion but a Lesson on Life" (p. 137). In failing to make that expected move to 
analysis, the student had just become a basic writer in the eyes of an institution. 

Why, we want to ask, are these students doing what seems to be the "wrong task" in the 
eyes of their readers -especially on these short assignments to which a grade or even placement is 
attached? Is it because they are unmotivated, despite the serious looks that suggest they, too, are 
disappointed, if not perplexed, by our response? Is standard "academic discourse" a new 
phenomenon to &U of these students? Isn't the assignment clear, even explicit? If our task involves 
reading to write, is it that they just haven't thought about the assigned readings deeply enough to 
have something to say? Somehow these answers don't do justice to the real effort made by both 
students and teachers. 

The phenomenon repeats itself in classes outside of English. Teachers ask students to attempt 
some "standard" form of disciplinary discourse because they want to expand the students 1 
repertoire by teaching a particular way of thinking and writing. Yet the class seems to be doing a 
variety of different assignments. Some of these variations are welcome inventions; but others 
suggest that the student is still confused about what academic discourse calls for. Writing about 
the problem of a rainy day probably seemed as trivial to the student who did it as it did to the 



instructor. The purposes of college were not being served for anyone. When writing goes awry in 
this way it is as though there were a band of writers each marching to a different drummer in die 
good faith that he or she was "doing what the assignment called for/ 1 

This study looks at one way we might help students learn to ui^erstand and manage the 
special demands of academic discourse. Part of the problem, we propose, may not lie in the 
students ability or even knowledge of the discourse per se, but in the way that student has 
construed the tisk. If this is true, students arc likely to have many abilities they could use if they 
prompted themselves to do so. This chapter will suggest that we may want to look at task 
representation not as a single, simple decision, but as an extended interpretive process that 
weaves itself throughout composing. The task as students represent it to themselves is, by 
definition, the one they perform, but that representation is subject to many influences and may 
evolve in surprising ways during writing . This process of task representation, we will suggest, 
may be far more involved, unpredictable and powerful than we have supposed. 

This problem of interpretation can be partly described in terms of the implicit requirements of 
academic discourse which students arc expected to infer. For instance, if we tell students that they 
need only look into their hearts and write from experience, are we assuming that the "personal 
essay" our field has in mind is simply a natural genre the student would discover by consulting 
those private wellsprings? Pat Bizzell has critiqued this assumption in textbooks that proport to 
teach "good" writing, when in fact they aim to teach a genre-specific form of good writing that has 
some quite explicit, if unarticulated rules (1986). Kathleen McCormick's critique of a naive use of 
"response statements" makes a similar point (1985). Response statements as she describes them 
are not pure, untrammeled "response" to a text. Rather, they arc a specialized form of discourse in 
which students are expected to use their personal response to examine their own reading process 
and assumptions about texts. Bazerman argues that the discourse conventions of the various 
disciplines pose a similar problem in writing across the curriculum (1981). The task of student 
writers is to enter the on-going intellectual conversation of an established community. 

Writing is a move in a discourse game with rules, an action in an intellectual and 
interpersonal context. Nevertheless, the process of interpreting i task-imagining the action that is 
called for- is sometimes equated with merely "following an assignment" and, as such, is relegated 
to remedial workshops on study skills. Teachers want to deal with heady intellectual processes, 
not with helping students ferret out "what the teacher wants," so they leave it to students to 
interpret assignments, even though the instructions may be long, and imaginative, and complex. 
Indeed, many teachers hold it as a badge of merit that they refuse to tell the students "what they 
want" in the desire to fos ter independence of thought. Yet there is sometimes a fine line between 
maintaining this proper reticence and creating a guessing game in which students who know how to 
succeed in school do, while those who don ! t are expected to infer it on their own. 

We find ourselves in a perplexing position. The genres we hold to be self-evident are not 
that way to everybody, after all. As Mina Shaughnessy has suggested, we seem to be urging 
some students, who do not know the "rituals and ways of winning arguments in academia, . . into 
the lion's den of academic disputation with no more than an honest face for protection " (1977, p. 
319). But this is not merely a problem of underprepared students. As we become more aware of 
the interpretive processes of readers and the multiple faces of academic discourse across 
disciplines, we must face the fact that students do intepret and may often misinterpret the college 
writing tasks they set out to do. If, indeed, the process of task representation plays the significant 
role I am suggesting, our problem is even more interesting. As a field we have almost nothing to 
say to students or each other about how writers represent tasks during composing and about the 
features of alternative representations students bring to any standard task. 

Task Representation 

Writing starts with a rhetorical situation that poses a need to write or, in the terms of this 
discourse, that poses a task that calls for problem-solving. Given that context, the first thing 
writers must do is define the problem or construct an image of that situation or task for themselves. 

? 

ER?C * 6 



We can think of this task theoretically as a problem space (Simon, 1973). This metaphoric space 
is made up of all the possible goals the solver might consider, all the possible operations, 
strategies, or moves that might be taken, and all the possible givens, or conditions that might 
constrain the solver f s action. The theoretical problem space for even everyday problems is often 
enormous, but, of course, we only work with those aspects of infinite possibility that we represent 
to ourselves. We solve the problem we construct. The process of task representation begins, as 
Figure 1 suggests, when the problem solver begins to represent the givens and constraints of this 
situation, the goals she would attain, and the strategies or actions she might take, since together 
these constitute the problem she is solving. 



AN UNFILLED PROBLEM SPACE 





Figure 1. The Content of a Problem Space 

For example, we can imagine two people thinking about the task of "planting beans." One 
person has a rather simple representation: 

The Givens & Constraints: Is it warm out there yet? Did I remember to order a picket 
of Kentucky Wonders from Park Seed? 

The Goals: Go out there; get 'em in; pick'em; and eat 'em. 

The Strategies: You make a row, sprinkle beans, stomp it down and pray for rain. 

We might compare this representation to that of Henry David Thoreau (see Figure 1), who also 
gave some thought to the task: 

The Givens & Constraints: Two and a half acres of upland, no equipment, and a 

3 



/ 

/ 

plentiful supply of hungry woodchucks were part of this reality. However, Thoreau often seemed 
to interpret his constraints as sources of value: "As I had little aid from horses or cattle, or hired 
men or boys, or improved implements of husbandry, I was much slower, and became much more 
intimate with my beans than usual (1964, p.406). 

The Strategies : Thoreau's strategies for growing beans suggest a task of somewhat more 
heroic dimensions than our first For instance, his habit is "to go daily to the rescue armed with a 
hoe, to thin the ranks of the enemy, to fill the trenches with the weedy dead." 

Hie Goals: Thoreau's goals also look oddly different. His agenda in planting a field of 
beans was nothing less than to "live deliberately" and to "know beans." It matters little, he said, to 
fill the farmer's barn. The goal of the husbandman, in Thoreau's remarkable vision of this task, is 
not to amass bushels but to "cease from anxiety." 

These two visions of planting beans may have little more in common than sunshine and bean 
seeds. And to a hardworking farmer in 1854 it is likely that neither of these representations woula 
seem sensible. Yet each has a logic and a rightness of its own. The point of this simpie example is 
two-fold: 

• First, a task is something people construct, even when they assume there is a common 
sense version everyone would hold. 

• Secondly, the levels of complexity within a given task can vary enormously. If this 
happens with beans, what happens with academic writing? 

Do students within the same class construct the same task for themselves? 

Does their image of the task resemble the image constructed by the instructor? 

And if these various representations differ, as on some level, of course, they must, do those 
differences really matter? Do they have a real impact on teaching, on learning, on succeeding in 
school? 

Cross cultural studies have given us some graphic examples of assignments and tests which 
were supposedly designed to test intelligence or cognitive capabilities (such as the power to 
abstract), but were in fact only describing task representation, that is, they were measuring the 
testee's assumptions about what a tester might want in posing such a peculiar task. Goodnow's 
(1976) review of the problems in interpreting cross-cultural research shows how this hidden 
variable of task representation can crop up in studies that intended to measure how literacy affects 
cognition. 

The "wise man/foolish man" phenomenen is a good example of this hidden influence. Our 
culture places a great value on the the ability to abstract and classify in certain ways (Arnheim, 
1954), and we often track the development of this ability as a measure of growth in writing ability 
(Britton, et a/., 1975, Freedman and Pringle, 1980). Investigators among the Kpelle people asked 
the Kpelle to classify a set of 20 familiar objects that (to our eyes) belonged in 4 categories: food, 
clothing, tools, and cooking utensils (Glick, 1975). The Kpelle persistently grouped them in 10 
groups of 2 based on concrete relations, e.g., "the knife goes with the orange because it cuts it." 
Should we conclude that these people do not carry out formal operations, that they lack these 
cognitive maneuvers? Glick (1975) however, noted: 

That subjects at times volunteered " 'that a wise man would do things in the way this 
was done.' When an exasperated experimenter asked finally, 'Ho* ✓ would a fool do 
it?* he was given back [groupings] of the type . . . initially expected-four neat piles 
with foods in one, tools in another" (p.636, quoted in Goodnow, p. 171). 

Closer to home, these studies have also taught us that certain general tendencies in 
representing tasks-which we take as a sign of intelligence or commitment— may also reflect 
culturally induced assumptions about how to handle a school task. 

Within our traditions, for instance, "learning by doing" and "learning from one's 
mistakes" are often acceptable, and guessing is usually expected. These are the 

traditions that make an early try feasible Groups such as the Navaho [by 

contrast] appear to rely on "prolonged observation, or 'prelearning' A 

4 

o 10 
ERLC 



V 

\ 

reluctance to try too soon and the accompanying fear of being 'shamed' if one does 
not succeed may account for the seemingly passive, uninterested, and unresponsive 
attitude of Indian students" (Ohannessian, 1967, p. 13, quoted from Goodnow, 
1976, p.181). 

Goodnow used this example to show how a "let's have a go at it" attitude toward school tasks can 
affect test taking. How, we might ask, would this image of school tasks affect students asked to 
generate tentative plans, notes, or drafts and then to revise? 

Labov came to much the same conclusion in Language in the Inner City when he attacked 
the myth that lower-class black children are verbally deprived and unable to deal with abstract, 
logically complex, or hypothetical questions (1972, p.220). The source of that myth, he argued, 
was in what we are calling task representation. Black children placed in settings without normal 
social support were asked to perform in ways they found mystifying, unmotivated (why should I 
tell you, an adult, that this is a space ship you are pointing to), or unreasonable (you want me to tell 
you about the fights I get into!?) Labov was able to show how changes in the context could elicit 
striking differences in performance-changes that he attributed to both motivation and the child's 
intepretation of the task. 

One can view these test stimuli as requests for information, commands for action, 
threats of punishment, or meaningless sequences of words. They are probably 
intended [by teachers or experimenters] as something altogether different-as 
requests for display, but in any case the experimenter is normally unaware of the 
problem of interpretation, (p.221) 

In these studies of discourse communities, of literate thinking, and of language use we can 
see not only the influence of context, but that that context is constantly being interpreted by 
language users. This study will try to add another piece to this picture of cognition in context by 
looking at the process of task representation itself. In particular, how do students handle this 
integrative process on standard college writing tasks? Are they any more cognizant of this 
intervening variable than we are as researchers and teachers? 

Although task representation may be an important decision, the process is often carried out 
with little or no awareness on the pan of the writer (Baker and Brown : 1984, Anderson, 1980). 
For familiar problems the process of representation is likely to be highly automated; it takes little 
conscious attention and the problem-solver may be reluctant to attribute any decision making or 
selective process to himself. He merely did what the assignment said. This feeling will be 
especially strong if the task invoked a well-developed schema, such as the schema for writing a 
thank-you note. Less familiar, more complex tasks, however, can call for extended exploration 
as a writer considers tentative, alternative ways to imagine the problem. In this process of 
interpreting a rhetorical situation, imagining what a reader would expect, gauging one's own 
feelings about a topic, envisioning ways to present a position, and even considering the meaning 
of terms in an assignment, writers are making crircaJ decisions. They arc setting goals and 
choosing actions that constitute a master plan and set of global instructions for how to approach 
this task. In an important study of the way social scientists approach problems in history and 
economics, Voss & aL (1983) found that experts created elaborated representations of a problem, 
features which they then tested and argued with as they worked toward a solution. This deliberative 
process, of course, takes time and effort; one wants to use one's highly automated processes 
whenever they will do the job. The problem in teaching is helping students learn to invoke 
conscious choice and evaluative awareness on complex problems that need them. Learning to 
manage academic discourse seems to be just such a problem. 

TASK REPRESENTATION IN READING-TO-WRITE: 
THE EXPLORATORY STUDY 

In the remainder of this chapter we will look in depth at the task representation process of 
students in a series of informal classroom experiments with undergraduate, masters and Ph. D. 

5 

O 11 

ERLC 



s 

I 

students, a scries we have labeled the Exploratory Study. This initial phase of the reading-to- write 
project opened up the territory for the more controlled observations of freshmen, labeled the 
Teaching Study and described in other reports in this series. The Exploratory Study is interesting 
for the hypotheses and questions it generated, for the template of alternative representations it 
provided, and for its rich picture of individual responses. That is where we will start 

It is not easy to understand, much less manage one's own composing process. Our 
knowlege of how this process operates in real time is often distorted by assumptions and 
conventional wisdom about how it should work and by the limits of our vocabulary for talking 
about the process. Because the romantic literary tradition, on which we depend for metaphors 
about writing, valued inspiration and talent over cognition and effort of mind, we may fail to 
appreciate the process of sustained thought tnat goes into normal writing. Even introspection, 
which is so essential to critical thinking and problem-solving, can be a blunt tool for uncovering 
cognitive processes, since people tend to recognize and remember those acts they expect and know 
well. Moreover, much of the cognition of writing, like that of any problem-solving act, is fleeting. 
People perform fascinating intellectual maneuvers, but once those maneuvers accomplish their end, 
thinkers wipe the me- *<*! slate, recalling only the result they struggled toward, and report that "it 
took a while, but Anally it just came to me" (Flower, in press). 

Process tracing experiments, as a form of classroom research, let students get a more vivid 
and accurate look at their own writing process (cf. Penrose, in prep.). As joint research projects 
carried out by teachers and students, they not only inform the teacher (as in traditional classroom 
research), but arc an important part of the course content for the stw/cnts. Both parties have an 
investment in discovery. 

The Exploratory Study began as a classroom experiment designed to look at the process of 
reading to write. For the junior, senior and graduate students involved, reading to write was a 
process worth examining. It was the mainstay of their college work and it was going to be as 
important on the job to the writing majors as it would be to the teachers. So I asked the class to do 
a small reading-to- write task and to collect a thinking-aloud protocol of themselves doing it That 
is, they were asked to think aloud to themselves as they read the source text and planned and wrote 
their short assignment, making a tape recording of the flow of their thoughts which they later 
transcribed (see Appendix 1). They could then use the protocol transcription to look more closely 
at their own process. A week later they returned to class to make a short presentation on "an 
interesting feature of my own process." (They would later do a short paper in which they applied 
what they learned to teaching, or professional writing, or themselves.) 

What I didn't tell them was that I had, in a sense, stacked the deck. In order to let them see 
as much of their own decision process as possible, the assignment was designed to simulate a 
typical, oper-ended, underspecified, and overloaded assignment- it asked for everything: read, 
interpret, synthesize, use all the "relevant" data, write your own statement and be comprehensive. 
On the other hand, by reciting all the sacred words of a standard college assignment, the 
assignment tried to be everything and hence nothing in order to let students catch a glimpse of how 
they were choosing u> represent this Rorschach blot to themselves. 

The assignment and source text (in Appendix 2) used with the English majors in this study 
was a series of quotes, notes and comments on the topic of revision. The goal was to create a short 
text (manageable in a classroom experiment), that simulated the experience of going to the library 
and reading a variety of sources with their distinctive voices and chdms, in order to write a paper 
of one's own. Some of the authors of the source texts disagree with one another, others are 
simply speaking at cross purposes. Some passages bury the relevant information on revision in a 
subordinate position to another, distractor topic. . Finally, there is no single issue or topic which 
organizes this set of notes; any ordering principle would have to come from the writer. 

Consider, if you will, how you would go about this task, were you a member of the class. 
In asking this question with various groups of teachers, I found per pie quite divided as to what 
the task requires. Some felt that the situation obviously called for summarizing. As a responsiMe 

6 

ERIC 12 



i 



writer, one would want to do justice to all the material here, reducing it to a concise and accurate set 
of gists organized around a central idea. Other teachers, looking mildly appalled at that prospect, 
said they would respond to the reading material as a springboard to writing about something they 
found personally relevant. And there were other responses. If wc found this diversity in how 
experienced teachers construed this reading-to-write task, what would students assume? And how 
would they go about the process of construing? 



TASK RE PRE SE NT A TION: OVERVIEW 



MAJOR SOURCE OF 
INFORMATION 

*Text 

* Text + My Comments 

* What I Already Knew 

* Previous Concepts +Text 



TEXT FORMAT AND 
FEATURES 

* Notes / Summary H 

* Summary + Opinions 

* Standard School Theme 

* Persuasive Essay 



ORGANIZING PLAN FOR WRITING 

To Summarize the Readings 

To Respond to The Topic 

To Review and Comment 

To Synthesize with a Controlling Concept 

To Interpret for a Purpose of My Own 



STRATEGIES 

* Gist & List 
Gist & List & Comment 
Read as a Springboard 
Tell it in My Own Words 
Skim & Respond 
Dig out an Organizing Idea 
Divide into Camps 

* Choose for Audience Needs 

* Use for My Own Purpose 



OTHER GOALS 

* Demonstrate understanding 

* Get a good idea or two 

* Present what ! learned 

* Come up with something interesting 
Do the minimum and do it quickly 
Fulfill the page requirement 
Test my own experience 
Cover all the key points 
Be original or creative 
Learn something for myself 
Influence the reader 
Test something I already knew 



Figure 2, Key Features of Students 1 Task Representations 



7 

13 



Over the course of these classroom experiments it became apparent that writers 1 top-level, 
global images of the task were regularly differing from one another on certain key features, which 
are presented as a menu of options in Figure 2. They were: the Major Source of Information, Text 
Format and Features, the Organizing Han, Strategies, and Goals. The analysis which follows will 
be organized around four issues that this diversity raised: 

1. The Power of the Organizing Flan 

2. How is a Task Representation Created? 

3. Costs, Benefits, Cognition and Growth 

4. Taking Metacognitive Control: Awareness vs. Standard Strategies 



1. THE POWER OF THE ORGANIZING PLAN 

A dominant feature of every writer's vision of this task was the organizing plan used to 
structure what was being read and to structure the writer's own text This organizing plan reflects 
a critical decision for a number of reasons. To begin with it guides the processes of reading and 
writing themselves. A plan to synthesize, for example, calls up certain strategies for manipulating 
ideas and transforming knowledge that a plan to summarize does not generally evoke. Hie 
organizing plan is also one of the bridges between process and product, since it dictates the 
organization of the written text . The logic of the organizing plan helps create an underlying 
coherent structure, visible in such places as introductory paragraphs, transitions, topic sentences, 
and conclusions. Because these structural features have such impact on the way instructors 
evaluate a paper, the organizing plan can influence students' grades. This is especially true if 
instructors value some organizing plans, such as synthesis or interpretation, as more intellectually 
significant than others, such as summary. If the choice of an organizing plan has such an impact 
on the process, product, and social outcomes of writing, what happens when students do not 
recognize the alternatives among which they are "choosing" or if they do not even realize that they 
have the option of choice? 

To illustrate the alternative plans this particular task elicited, I will draw on the presentations 
students made as part of the process experiments and on their responses to one another. 

The Organizing Plan to Summarize 

Martha was a good student in a quiet, dutiful, straightforward sense of the term. She was an 
engineering student in her junior year, nervous about writing in general and about this class of 
English majors and MAs in particular, but used to succeeding and getting things right She was 
also very clear about how to do this task. She used what we later began to call the gist and list 
strategy. In her view, you read through the text with some care, find the key words in each 
paragraph, and summarize it trying to capture its main idea* You then write your paper around this 
string of well-wrought gists. An important caution goes along with this plan: sometimes a new 
idea occurs to you as you are writing-a different way to organize or an idea you are interested in. 
If that happens, don't be led astray. You must decisively set that idea aside, for it will only confuse 
you and your paper. 

In examining and describing her own process, Martha had uncovered for herself a 
well-honed strategy that she obviously relied on for other assignments. This task was, she 
concluded in her presentation, "just like doing a research paper. " If we respond to this plan as 
"mere" summary, it is important for us to realize that Martha did not see it as a limited or low effort 
choice. She was very serious about her work and serious about this presentation. She was 
describing the task as she saw it, and as (I believe) she assumed everyone else saw school writing. 

The dynamics of this class are themselves a part of the story of this research. By a stroke of 

8 



14 



fortune, Martha's presentation had been the very first in the two-day series of talks on what each 
student discovered. Her well-defined vision of the task, which we began to call the efficiency 
expert strategy, created a backdrop against which other representations took shape. In fact, a 
dialogue began with the very next presentation. That writer, Kate, was an economics major at the 
beginning of her MA in professional writing. Since she seemed to approach everything with Irish 
energy to spare, it didn't seem so surprising when she said that it never occurred to her to use the 
source text in the way Martha had. In fact, the interesting feature from her protocol was the way 
the topic of the paper itself had been determined by her sense of an imagined audience. She 
decided cm relevant content information by first imagining an audience of students she had once 
tutored and then deciding what they might want to hear about revision. This was such a standard 
strategy for her, she told us later, that she even had a mental formula for it: T= f(A). Topic is a 
function of the audience. 

I will return to Kate's vision of this task later. The point of my narrative digression here is 
simply to convey the impact this sequence of presentations had on all of us. The question of right 
or wrong was temporarily in abeyance since the point of the experiment was to uncover something 
interesting about one's own strategies on this small experimental task. What became increasingly 
clear was that people in the room were holding radically different representations of the task and 
relying on strategies that would inevitably produce very different papers. Yet each assumed he or 
she was simply doing the task. Let me briefly sketch the other dominant organizing plans that 
emerged from these visions of the task. 

The Organizing Plan to Respond To/Write About the Topic 

In sharp contrast to the summarizing plan, some students were not inconvenienced by the 
assigned text, because they choose to talk about what they already knew. They used the reading as 
a springboard to trigger their own ideas or response to the topic in general. Notice that this is a 
more freewheeling plan than one which would require responding directly to the claims in the 
source text This plan for reading and writing can produce excellent themes which are 
well-organized and unusually interesting because they are based on ideas the student has already 
thought about and is already motivated to consider. In many writing classes, this is exacdy the task 
teachers want writers to give themselves. On the other hand, this task, like summarizing, sidesteps 
the process of integrating one's own knowledge with that of the source text It typically simplifies 
the process of reading-to- write. 

Our best insight into this plan came from a student's description of a skim and respond 
strategy which she discovered in her protocol and came to recognize as one of her mos 
dependable strategies for generating text The example from her protocol begins with Janet reading 
a line from the source text (underlined in the excerpt below), followed by a brief period of thinking, 
and then composing a sentence (in italics). In Janet's description of this process, the sentence she 
composed came tumbling out with the energy of a discovery. Notice, however, the relation 
between the ideas about revision in the source text and those in Janet's claim. 

Good writers check to see if plans hav e changed mid-stream (source textV Urn, I 
guess, let's see, your first thoughts are usually muddled or come out like a tidal 
wave. The tidal wave effect The tidal wave effect of a rush of initial 
ideas or thoughts can be cleaned up and clarified on revision (draft 
text). 

Janet's strategy was to skim the source text, waiting for those points which would trigger a 
response and give her something interesting to say. In her example, the term "mid-stream" (and the 
notion of change?) seems to have triggered the idea of tidal waves. The link is a lexical rather than 
propositional one; the two texts are not even talking about the same subject A series of inferences 
and associations based on the surface of the source text led Janet to generate her own idea. In her 
presentation, Janet was intrigued with this discovery because <Ms was the strategy that had gotten 
her through college. It had been especially good for English courses, she reported; she had once 
written an entire paper on a word in Shakespeare. 



9 



As the course went on, it became clear that Janet relied on this local skim and respond 
strategy so heavily because she, like a few other students in the class, also depended on the larger 
plan of Responding to the Topic to oiganize her reading and writing. In the attempt to find 
something interesting to say, the substance of the source text served primarily as a springboard for 
thought or trigger for past associations. And in Janet's version of the strategy, sometimes only the 
words of the source mattered 

As students talked on, a vision of the costs and benefits associated with these different 
organizing plans began to emerge. This is one of the critical issues of task representation we will 
return to, but Janet and Martha illustrate the basic question. Martha's gist and list strategy is highly 
efficient, a very intelligent plan for many tasks. On the other hand, this summarizing task 
eliminates the possibility of exploring or expressing one's own ideas. And it is probably not the 
task instructors have in mind on many college assignments. Janets plan of Responding to the 
Topic had apparently stood her in good stead in some undergraduate literature classes. But in my 
class slie was having genuine difficulty with assignments which asked for a sustained argument 
and focused analysis of an issue. Our exploration of alternative organizing plans and the strategies 
which supported them were raising the question: what are the costs and benefits for the writer 
which these different representations carry with them? 

The Organizing Plan to Review and Comment 

Many students took a middle ground between summarizing and abandoning the source texts. 
They carried on what one student called a "dialogue" with their sources in which they would 
alternate between reviewing or summarizing a source and then adding their own comments, 
criticisms, or associations. A more formal version of this plan was one many students had done in 
high school in which the writer summarizes a source and then adds an "opinion paragraph" at the 
end. 

This plan not only allowed writers to express their own ideas, it led to an easy said natural 
way to compose, since the text could be structured like a conversation built on the scaffolding of 
the source text This ease was also its limitation as a plan for thinking or persuading. The review 
and comment plan did not lead the writer to pursue connections or conflicts or to build an 
integrated picture of a topic. 

Since this plan begins to assume major importance when we turn to the work of freshmen, 
we will return to it later. The final two plans we observed are distinguished by the prominent role 
they give to integration of 
ideas. 

The Organizing Plan to Synthesize Ideas around a Controlling Concept 

Some writers gave themselves an additional set of goals that went beyond summarizing or 
reviewing and commenting on the text They saw their task as organizing information, from both 
the source and themselves, under a controlling, synthesizing concept. Unlike the summarizes, 
they read the the source text with an eye to uncovering a unifying thread or to creating one, and 
they tried to organize their own texts around this central concept. And unlike the students who 
saw the task as responding to the topic, the synthesizers made themselves responsible for (at least 
some of) the ideas in the source texts. 

Given the wide range of meanings people assign to the term synthesis, we want to be 
precise about the way it is used here. In this study, a text with a "synthesizing plan" was 
operationally defined as having these features: 

1) It offered the reader a clearly articulated "synthesizing concept" which one could actually 
locate in the text 

2) This concept was a substantive, informative idea rather than an immediately obvious 
inference. For instance, a text which stated that "there are many opinions on revision" and 
proceeded to summarize the sources was not held to be governed by a unique "synthesizing" 

10 



V 

\ 

concept. 

3) Finally, this concept not only appeared in the text, it worked as a controlling concept that 
governed the selection of information and the organization of the entire text. A synthesis must have 
a controlling concept that indeed controls. 

To us as instructors the benefits of this organizing plan may seem particularly striking. To 
begin with, it encourages thinking processes which learning theorists and reading educators would 
want to foster. It asks the student to read source texts for ideas at the level of gists not details 
(Brown and Day, 1986), to generate his or her own macro-level structure of ideas (Meyer, 
1982), and to integrate this information into a meaningful, memorable whole (Ausaubel, 1963) 
which assimilates or accomodates itself to one's prior knowledge (Piaget, 1932). The plan to 
synthesize also has obvious benefits for the writing process insofar as it would produce a clear 
organizing idea, a structured integration of various sources, and the opportunity to place one's self 
in an intellectual discourse, combining one's own ideas with those of other authors (Spivey, 1984). 
(This is not to say all syntheses achieve this, but the plan leads in that direction.) 

However, this plan also carries some very real costs. To begin with, it was not clear even to 
these generally successful college students what synthesis meant, when it got down to actually 
doing it For one student, this quandry about the task was in fact the "interesting feature" of her 
own process as she found herself rereading the assignment and puzzling out what to do. As she 
found herself saying in the protocol, 

Interpret and synthesize 1 [re-reading the assignment]. What the hell does that 
mean? Synthesize means to pull together, no, to make something up. Why should I 
want to make something up? [She then re-reads and comments on the wording of the 
assignment] Synthesis sounds like Tm making a chemical compound Hma Put 
together. [Re-reads] "All of the relevant findings in the text" How can I do this? 

And, in essence, this writer concluded that she couldn't or didn't, in fact, want to "make something 
up" and began at this point to summarize, saying, 

"Ok, I know everything about these few pieces of writing, about [reading from the 
assignment] how people revise. Okay, 111 write this down.... How people 
revise. 

Other writers set out with the goal of finding the unifying thread in the sources they read. 
However, we had designed even this short text to replicate the experience of normal reading 
(outside of textbooks). These authors not only disagree but focus on different aspects of the topic 
so that their main ideas resist falling into neat packages. For instance, how do the rather bare facts 
about business writers connect to the enthusiastic claims by teacher* that good writers do extensive 
revision? One writer, who spent between two and three hours on the task, found himself caught in 
an extended struggle with his plan since the sources suggested two major, alternative organizing 
ideas, yet neither concept was supported enough in the sources to allow a clear choice. The 
comments here were separated in time and are numbered to reflect a sequence of points at which 
this writer was encountering the costs of attempting this organizing plan. 

1 . Uhhh, so what's the contrast here? The contrast seems to be between people who are 
experienced writers, versus students. Aaahhh, so wait let's see, [rereads text to see if 
students mentioned in the text were also labeled inexperienced]. 

2. Essentially the entire passage is oriented towards[...] probably about 75% of it is about 
editing and revision [...] and the remaining 25% is about planning. Planning is tied into 
revision because writers review their plans and goals, ...and then, and then hov; is that tied 
into revision? 

3a. [ Rereads assignment 1... Statement about the process of revision 

3b. Well, what have we got? What's the process? Well, revision is part of the process of 

11 



17 



4 



4a. Well, we have two main axes , two main axes of organization here... 

4b. As in my notes we have, uhhh, we have good writers versus bad writers uhhhh, 

[looks at notes] vertically and horizontally... 

4c. And various processes of revision in center stage [ of his notes]. 

4d. So there is another potential organization to this paper. 

Andrew's experience was not unique. The first hurdle for some students was recognizing 
that this information by "authorities" did not easily resolve itself into convenient, obvious, or even 
reasonable packages. They had trouble building a meaningful version of the source text(s) in their 
own minds. The next hurdle was forging a synthesis. Students who wanted 1) to base this 
synthesis entirely on the text and do justice to most of the sources and yet 2) to make claims they 
felt were supported by evidence, found themselves under enormous constraints. They had, I 
believe we would agree, given themselves a task that was simply undoable, given the information 
the source provided Although many of these students questioned the assignment, few questioned 
their own representation of this task* 

Students who attempted to create a synthesis based on a concept found in the source were 
far more likely to meet failure and have to revise their plan, even though most did so with some 
understandable reluctance. As one student said, "I don't like to think of myself as abandoning an 
idea." But when her attempt to neatly categorize the authorities failed she felt forced to change the 
plan and organize the essay around her own impressions. And that decision carried its own costs 
as she moved from finding , testing, ana using a concept to generating, testing and using one. 

The point here is that this plan carries very real intellectual cc- ts. It can be difficult, 
frustrating and chancy. The source texts, one's own knowledge, arid reality itself may resist 
synthesis. And there is always the practical question: is "making something up" even called for 
here; why would I want to do that? Representing the task in this way is a meaningful choice. 

The Organizing Plan to Interpret or Use Ideas for a Rhetorical Purpose 

Synthesis is an intellectually sophisticated endeavor. Because it asks the writer to 
re-organize and integrate information around a controlling concept, it is one of the mainstays of 
academic writing, especially of student academic writing. However, a synthesis, as we defined it, 
is primarily focused on conveying a structured body of information. It is rather like a standard 
textbook. Some of the writers we observed added a rhetorical dimension to their task by 
attempting to interpret their information in order to carry out a rhetorical purpose. ( In using the 
t i rhetorical purpose here we mean a purpose that goes beyond the goals of exposition defined 
as presenting a summary, comment, or synthesis). 

We observed the presence of an active rhetorical purpose in three places: 

1 . During the composing process, some writers spent time attending to the audience or to 
their own interests and setting goals for what might be interesting or useful to do in this paper. 
These rhetorical goals-beyond exposition-helped dictate not only what information the writer 
would use and how it was ordered, but what organizing or synthesizing concept would control the 
text and why. 

2. In some texts, there was a discernable rhetorical purpose organizing the essay, which 
took the form of making a claim, posing a question, or setting up an issue ( e.g., "Is there really 
one good revision process, or does "good" depend on the kind of writing?"). The text functioned 
as a way to explore that question, to articulate sides of a debate, or to come to a conclusion. As a 
reader of these texts one had a sense of being involved in a guided inquiry that had a rhetorical 
purpose that went beyond conveying information. 

3. In other texts the discernable rhetorical purpose took the form of addressing a particular 
reader and adapting the writer's knowledge to what that reader might need. These texts were often 
organized as advice to students or as plans for putting the research on revision to use. 



This category of "interpret for a rhetorical purpose" raised some worthwhile problems of 

18 



definition for us that might be useful to explore here. To begin with, we need to recognize the 
ways a rhetorical purpose as we will define it differs from other related purposes. A teacher, for 
example, has an educational purpose in asking students to write a synthesis or interpretation-the 
purpose of the assignment is to teach these valued skills. A given student, on the other hand, may 
have an equally important purpose quite at odds with that assignment This writer's personal 
purpose for writing may lead him to summarize as a way of thinking over and remembering what 
he found intriguing in William James. For that student, summary may have seemed the best 
personal use to which his writing could be put, regardless of the assignment. Purpose is also 
inherent in many textual conventions and genres (e.g., we could say the writer's purpose was to 
produce a summary, a synthesis, a description or some other conventional form). The rhetorical 
purpose to which we refer here is, by contrast, a set of goal s, unique to this text, which functions 
for reader and writer as an explicit organizing feature of the discourse. (E.g., Kate's purpose was 
to get the students she had tutored to consider certain revision strategies she thought they didn't 
use.) The papers which fell into this final category were adapting, transforming, and integrating 
information from sources and knowledge in order to cany out a discernable rhetorical purpose in a 
unique piece of discourse. 

Looking for a rhetorical purpose (as we have defined it) raised a second problem. Is the text 
a student produces always an adequate guide to the presence or absence of rhetorical purpose in the 
writer's own thinking? ( Later parts of this study explored this important issue in some depth. Cf. 
Stein and Peck.) In our observations of the composing process many writers gave no apparent 
attention to matters of audience or purpose— beyond the conventional purpose of conveying 
information. In this case, the a-rhetoricai, information centered plan that organized texts of these 
writers seemed to be an accurate reflection of their process and purpose. On the other hand, some 
students gave a variety of indications during composing, during revising, and in discussion that 
they did indeed have a rhetorical purpose in mind which was exerting an influence on their reading 
and interpretation of the source text However, in a subset of these cases even when the protocols 
revealed an active sense of purpose, our independent raters saw little or no indication of interpretive 
purpose in the student's paper. From the reader's point of view there was no discernable rhetorical 
purpose controlling the text; the reader had not been brought into the discourse. We wonder if this 
phenomenon, which Peck describes as the "Intenders," is a common one. As students learn to 
manage academic discourse, they may be actively engaging in rhetorical thinking and trying new 
strategies of transforming knowledge before they are able to use that purpose to control a text. The 
reader may be the last to know. Teachers, it follows, may need to attend to both the process and 
the text if they want to see a student's development. 

Final!/, this category led us to ask a third question: Is synthesis really possible without a 
rhetorical purpose, (regardless of whether that purpose is discernable in the text)? Scaidamalia and 
Berieter (1987) have suggested that children only move beyond what they call a 
"knowledge-telling" strategy to a "knowledge-transforming" strategy when they are able to rise to 
rhetorical planning . In this view it is the influence of other readers and other goals that moves 
children to transform or reconstruct their knowledge. Composition texts often assume that the best 
way to teach and prompt writers to synthesize their knowledge in new or creative ways is to 
introduce the needs of an audience. This pedagogical assumption is probably right. On the other 
hand, both the protocols and the texts show us rigorous, extended efforts at synthesis which are 
not driven by a unique rhetorical purpose but by an attempt to construct a coherent, meaningful, or 
comprehensive synthesis of information. The writer's purpose is defined by the conventions of 
the genre. 

Carrying out a unique rhetorical purpose, we would suggest, calls for strategies of 
knowledge transformation that go beyond those required for synthesis (or other 
conventionally-defined purposes). Texts organized by a rhetorical purpose, of course, often have a 
synthesis, summary, or other plan embedded within thent But beyond that, organizing around a 
purpose asks the writer to define and often redefine this purpose itself, to then use it to organize or 
infer relevant information, and finally to make that purpose a discernable, controlling feature of the 
text in a way that invites the reader into the discussion. (We should note that this strategy of 
inviting the reader into the inquiry and revealing one's purpose is a feature of academic discourse, 

13 

IS 



t 

not of rhetorical plans in general, such as those which underlie advertising, editorials, etc.) 

Writers 1 unique rhetorical purposes can, by definition, take a variety of forms. Gary 
initially saw his task as using the research on revision to understand himself and (implicitly) to 
judge whether he was a good writer or not He read each section in order to fit it into a private 
thesis he was building about what a "good writer " should do arid at the same time to apply those 
"shoulds" to himself. Since he normally did not revise at all, this comparison and the conflict it 
engendered took up a large chunk of his reading and thinking- it invoked strategies for comparing, 
testing ideas and evaluating that did not appear in other protocols. However, this effort was only 
apparent in the process data, not the written product, in which he chose to review the sources. The 
unique purpose which he used for private inquiry was not used as an organizing plan for the text, 
but it is a good example of a purposeful plan. 

Gary's rhetorical purpose was in a sense directed to an audience of one. But unlike a 
"response to the topic" plan, it required him to use the source text, to interpret it in light of a 
question he posed, and to apply that information to the specific rhetorical purpose of comparing his 
practice with the "shoulds" he inferred from his sources. 

Kate, the writer we referred to earlier, created a rhetorical purpose that involved readers 
and put even more demands on her reading-to- write process. Like a few other students in the 
class, Kate apparently assumed that even if an audience wasn f t specified, it made sense to create 
one. So from early on in the process, she approached this material as potential advice for students 
she had known when she was a peer tutor. The interesting feature of her own process was the way 
this plan to advise helped her "bring out meaning." The strategies she described were ones that 
also turned up in the work of synthesizers: she tried to link the data in the text with her own 
experience. In order to select the important ideas to include, she used criteria such as, is this claim 
supported by lots of points in the source text, does it make a big difference, is it part of a 
controversy? The task of synthesizing was apparently embedded in her representation (just as the 
task of summarizing is often embedded in synthesizing). However, the larger plan which gave 
direction to her reading and writing process and gave structure to her text was guided by her 
rhetorical purpose. It was this purpose which let her put synthesis to work for a rhetorical end. 

We have elaborated on this rhetorical plan because our experience suggests that students 
entering academic discourse seem less prepared for this rhetorical task than for summary, 
synthesis, or personal response. They seem, for instance, less prepared to develop their own goals 
in interaction with a source and often unaware that a text can be a good synthesis but a failure at 
achieving its purpose or adapting information for a reader. Students who do not recognize that 
rhetorical plans exist as a distinct option in academic discourse might also have trouble interpreting 
college assignments that call for original thinking or imaginative application of course material to a 
new problem. 

A plan dominated by a rhetorical purpose has an important but unclear status in academic 
writing. It is clearly a risky proposition in doing assigned writing--you can get off the track and 
fulfill a purpose that is not shared by your instructor, especially if the instructor wants you to 
concentrate on the reading material itself or on the formal features of a genre, such as a critical 
essay or historical analysis. A rhetorical plan can make the writer's process a highly selective one, 
since the "relevant data" is that which contributes to the purpose. Although a rhetorical purpose 
involves using the source text, it may do so in idiosyncratic ways for purposes that the instructor 
finds surprising or not "relevant" to the purpose of the assignment. Furthermore, to use this plan, 
one must not only develop a purpose but transform information to fulfill it This representation of a 
reading-to-write task as a rhetorical task has the potential for real costs. 

On the other hand, this vision of one's task has considerable benefits. From a pragmatic 
point of view, it is a good basis for building an argument and for answering questions in college 
courses that ask you to manipulate ideas rather than just organize and recall them. It is also the plan 
that guides much of the reading-to-write that adults outside of school do when they read reports, 
instructions, or memos and write the same for their own purposes. In the larger scheme of 

14 



20 



education t seeing writing as a rhetorical act and seeing the text as an instrument of purpose is an 
entry point into critical literacy. It is a plan that favors guts, maturity, and independence of mind as 
well as sensitivity to the response of the reader one is talking to. It allows students to treat the 
ideas and texts of others (including those of "authorities") as well as their own knowledge and texts 
as open to scrutiny and transformation. Facts can be not only tested but interpreted and put to 
different uses; claims and concepts can be evaluated and responsibly transferred to new settings. 
Using reading and writing for a rhetorical purpose brings together ihe receptive goals of literacy 
with both the testing and transforming actions of critical literacy. 



2. HOW IS A TASK REPRESENTATION CREATED? 

The Organizing Plan described above stood out as a dominant feature of this task, but it was 
not the only important feature on which students differed. As Figure 2 shows, writers on this task 
made decisions that differed clearly from one another in five areas. They made chores about 
where their Information would come from (i.e., the source text, their own ideas on the subject, 
both, or concepts and previously structured information imported to this discussion ). They made 
choices about the Text Format and Features t hat seemed appropriate (i.e., informal notes or 
summary, summary paragraphs] plus opinion[s] of the sort encouraged in high school; a standard 
school theme; or the sort of persuasive essay one sees in acadeinic and professional writing). They 
made, as we have seen, choices about the Organizing Plan which guided both the reading/writing 
process and the plan of the text In these first three areas, Information, Format and Organizing 
Plan, we have tried to present the major choices observed For the last two areas, Strategies and 
Goals, we can hope only to present a suggestive list of the various strategies students described 
and the even wider set of goals they reported, which ranged from being creative to being 
comprehensive and accurately representing what they had learned 

The menu of options in Figure 2 is not the only way to categorize a writing task; it is a 
reflection of this particular task and necessarily incomplete at that What it did for us was function 
as a backdrop against which we could observe both individual differences and the unfolding 
process of task representation itself. Given these five areas as important sites of decision and 
difference, when and how are those decisions made? 

On the basis of these and later observations, we can propose a tentatr * theory of task 
representation as a constructive process, organized around three principles: 

1. The task a writer constructs is not a simple choice, but the integration of a set of options 

and sehcmaSi 

This principle makes two points. One is that an image of a task is not created de novo, but 
depends on the schemas, conventions, patterns, and strategies the writer already knows. 
However, these options are stored in many independent pockets of knowledge which must be 
integrated afresh for each new task. To present the notion of choice among options in the teaching 
phase of this study, we borrowed a metaphor from personal computers in which users must 
select their commands from a set of options offered by "pull-down menus." The menu in Figure 3 
illustrates how computer users create the specs for the task the computer is to perform by visually 
"pulling down" a menu of standard options and selecting the features they want This menu can be 
likened to a sub-set of discourse features which typically defue a given writing task. This menu 
shows a set of "*Me" decisions a Macintosh user must make. Another menu asks for "format" 
decisions involving spacing, headers, and footers and so on. As you will note, each of these 
menus usually provides a default choice that is invoked automatically if one makes "no choice." 
On the Macintosh menu for "style," the default choice is 12 point type and plain text. This default 
decision guides the computer if the writer is not in active control of die process. 




Chapt kPlain Tent 



XI 

no 
m 

XL 



Bald 



ite/ic 



Underlir.a 

I (DMGGD9 



Superscript 



Subscript 



9 Point 

10 Point 
✓12 Point 

14 Point 
18 Point 
24 Point 



SELECTING OPTIONS FROM A MENU 
If I don't bother to select, this is what I get- 12 point type, plain text. 
But I think this job calls for LARGE TYPE. 
INDENTATIONS 

LARGER TYPE? OR MAYBE BOLD FACE. 
HOW ABOUT A SIHAKDW ? 
& UNDERLINING FOR EMPHASIS. 



This analogy to a computer menu highlights two aspects of a constructive process that are 
easy to overlook. One is the sheer number of distinct menus or areas in which writers are making 
choice* -whether they realize it or not-as well as the range of choices to be made within these 
aress. (Here, the computer analogy fails to capture the additional possibility of creating a new or 
unique option, unless we import a programmer into our story.) Secondly, the computer analogy 
shows how using the default option (e.g., using one's "standard" approach to a paper assignment) 
can be an efficient way to bypass problem-solving and leap in with familiar strategies. However, 
that happy leap doesn't eliminate the fact that a real choice, on a much fuller menu, was in fact 
made. Unexamined decisions made by default are still decisions. 

Are there ways writers can streamline this decision proce - .;? Clearly leaping in with default 



Figure 3. Options on a Computer Pull-Down Menu 



options and "standard" strategies can make some of the choices easy. Our initial question was, do 
these choices fall into packages, in which a given Organizing Plan, say, was always associated 
with a given set of choices about Information, Format, Strategies and Goals? This would suggest 
that writers had well-formed schemas for entire tasks of this sort, which they simply invoked, 
rather than actively constructed 

To some extent this appeared to be true of summarizing--it is strongly tied to the original text 
as its Information source and to gist and list Strategies, and yet the format summarizers chose did 
vary and their reasons ranged from showing learning to getting done quickly. This hypothesis, 
that writers are simply selecting among a set of ready-made schemas at the level of the entire task, 
nn into even greater difficulty on two grounds. First was the striking individual variation in the 
choices writers said they were making here and in Ackerman's later analysis of the freshmen (see 
Ackerman). Despite some predictable trends in these patterns (e.g., summarizing paired with gist 
and list), knowing a writer's organizing plan did not allow a reliable prediction about the Format or 
the Information source much less about Strategies and Goals. 

Yet, if there are no general, shared schemas for this task, perhaps individual writers possess 
their own personal task schemas that they regularly invoke for school tasks. Although this 
hypothesis found some support, and writers did talk of their "standard" strategies, these students 
also talked of confusion, uncertainty, and conflicts within their own image of what to do. 
Students, it appears, may have standard strategies and partial schemas, but not have an integrated 
image of the entire task. This is not, we believe, because these were novice writers, but because 
representing complex writing tasks is by nature a constructive process. The next two principles 
attempt to account for the more active constructive process we observed even on this relatively 
simple task. 

2. Because the process of constructing a task representation depends on noticing cues fmrr\ 
the context and evoking relevant mem ories, it can extend over the course of composing. 

Decisions students made about tht Ave features of the task in Figure 2 appeared to be made at 
different times and for different reasons. Some decisions came out of planning or reviewing 
episodes. But others were the result of an opportunistic move. For example, some students started 
with the apparent plan to use the source text, until an interesting idea or inference changed the pool 
of information. This choice on the writer's Information "menu" was dictated by a local event, not 
by an initial, integrated vision of the task. Nor was it dictated by a conscious decision that a 
commentary paper would be more appropriate than a summary. A lucky event in the reading 
process determined a piece of the plan. 

The schematic diagram in Figure 4 sketches the cognitive processes that could account for 
these observations and predict some of the problems these writers encountered. As other 
research has shown, Planning and Reviewing are both powerfully generative processes (Flower, 
et al. y in prep; Hayes, et al 1987). However, here we wish to emphasize the role "noticing" and 
"evoking" can play in shaping the task writers give themselves. 



17 

23 



CONTEXT 

Assignment, class, school, etc. 



I 




THE PROCESS 





Noticing J 
Evoking 


1 












Planning 




Reviewing 



Updating the Image 



THE 
REPRESEN 
TATION 



Current 



Revised 
Current 



Figure 4. Noticing and Evoking Within the Process of Task Representation 



We can read this diagram as charting a set of possible loops in the process of constructing a 
representation. In one loop, the writer's current representation of the task is about to be changed 
by the process of noticing or evoking. We begin with the current TASK REPRESENTATION 
Gocated metaphorically in the box on the right) which at this moment consists of the major goals, 
constraints and strategies currently activated in the writer's thinking. The PROCESS of 
constructing (in the center box) is re-initiated in this case when the writer notices the word 
"synthesize" in the assignment (from the CONTEXT box). Noticing in turn leads her to search 
Memory and evoke a little package of relevant information on the subject (she doesn't know a lot 
about synthesis so this is a small addition). Or, in another loop, it occurs to her that her current 
plan to summarize is turning out to be rather boring ( i.e., she "notices" and reflects on her own 
current task representation) and this sends her back to the assignment and the Context of writing 
(i.e., what am I supposed to do?) or back into Memory and Planning (i.e., what else could I do; 
what have I done before?). Or, finally, our writer might make an effort to evoke the memory of 
what was said in class or how the instructor typically responds to original ideas as information 
about what is possible on this task. 

18 

• 24 



This noticing and evoking process is unlike the act of selecting a more or less complete 
schema for a task from memory. It is responsive to cues from the context of writing, to memory, 
and to evocative features of the current task itself-cues which may pop up at any point as more 
information is assembled or new possibilities open up ( cf. Simon, 1973). It can go on during 
reading and writing without the writer's conscious control. However, it can also lead to a very 
goal-directed search of both the context and the writer's knowledge and an active period of 
Planning or Reviewing. 

Noticing doesn't guarantee a change. The information this process generates can, of course, 
be lightly considered and ignored, or it can be used to update the current representation into a 
revised current representation. This act of updating one's image is an interpretive act For 
instance, how will our student translate her instructor's habit of closely questioning students' 
claims (a cue from the context) into a constraint, a goal, or strategy for this task? 

The constructive process may continue as result of this updating if it sends the writer into 
more planning and setting new goals. A revised representation that now includes "synthesis," for 
example, may call for a substantial new plan of action from the writer. 

This small model lets us describe operationally: (1) how the process of noticing and evoking 
links context, memory and the writer's representation, (2) how this process can run on its own 
steam throughout composing, so that task representations evolve over time-even after the writer 
has a plan underway-and (3) how an interpretive act (the "updating") that stands between noticing 
and having a revised image can lead to some of the problems we observed. We will turn to those 
now. 



3. Developments and changes in a writer's re presentation can lead to p roblems, in 
constructing an integrated task and fc^, 

As the diagram shows, the representation that an individual writer constructs over the 
course of producing the paper is not always stable. Writers in this study tried a variety of 
strategies: a successful move might suggest an organizing plan; an unsuccessful effort might lead 
to changes in the plan. This fluidity was not always welcome. Recall the student who "didn't like 
to think of herself as abandoning an idea" and dropped her initial plan to do a summary with a five 
paragraph theme format only when she was unable to neatly categorize the "authorities." Forced to 
come up with her own ideas for making sense of the sources, but clinging to the theme format, she 
finally resolved the dilemma by structuring the theme around a comparison of her ideas and those 
jf the authorities. For her, getting the parts of her plan synchronized was the central event in her 
process. 

As the noticing and evoking model suggests, a writer's image of the task can change in a 
piecemeal fashion. A new idea added late in the game may conflict with a goal set earlier, but the 
lack of integration can remain unseen. This may be one reason inexperienced writers end up with 
texts that appear to have been written by different hands. Looking at their protocols, a number of 
students discovered to their surprise that they had made dramatic shifts in parts of their 
representation part way through the task. For instance, one experienced student writer plunged 
into audience analysis and developed an interesting rhetorical plan around her readers-a plan which 
was elaborated in the protocol and evident in the text However, in analyzing her own protocol 
Ruth discovered that near the end of writing, she seemed simply to forget this reader-based plan 
and switched to what she characterized as a standard theme and summary of information. The 
voice and viewpoint she had been cultivating was dropped, the diction changed and the paper ended 
on an unexpectedly lame note. Her inventive plan for the task had been replaced, without her 
awareness, by a simpler, doubtless more familiar, plan and practiced strategy, which yielded an 
oddly disjointed text. Yet we might speculate that it passed her own rev? jw because each section 
did fit different parts of the plan she had in mind. 



A second student illustrates how this ongoing constructive process and its range of options 

19 

erJc 25 



/ 

can lead to internal conflicts for a writer. Ann began with the apparently unquestioned assumption 
that she had to cover all the information in the source texts. As she worked on this goal, an internal 
critic would burst in and criticize her work for not being original and creative. She wanted to do 
a thorough synthesis, but became repeatedly disillusioned when the work so far did not also yield 
an insight that was personally relevant to herself or to the students for whom she wanted to write. 
The task she represented wis not only unmanagcable-her internal critic had a low tolerance for 
anything shot of brilliance even during idea generation -but its plan for both inclusive synthesis 
and marked originality was in conflict with itself. This writer later mentioned that she had never 
turned in a major paper on time during her college career. 

The questions raised by these observations are ones that this study can not fully answer. 
However, if the constructive hypothesis is correct, task representation may be both creative and 
difficult to manage precisely because it is an extended process and because tasks are not simply 
' selected, 11 in the way old-fashioned textbooks used to tell students merely to select a thesis. 
Because this representation is constructed as the process of reiding-to- write goes along, the 
opportunity for choice and revision of choice carries with it the chance of disjointed texts and 
conflicting plans. 

3. COST, BENEFITS, COGNITION, AND GROWTH 

Because we are educators, any analysis of organizing plans or text types is going to raise 
questions of value and assumptions about the difficulty or ease, the sophistication or simplicity of 
different plans. This analysis is no exception and we would like to consider and question three 
common assumptions one might use to rank these plans or choose which one to use or to teach. 
Each of these assumptions conbines a persuasive element of common sense with a faith that the 
forms and modes of discourse (in this case organizing plans) are reliable indicuors of 
sophistication, cognition or growth. It woulu be immensely convenient to the educational 
establishment if this belief were tnie. The first assumption we wish to question is that synthesis 
and interprctaton are nore valuable approaches to take to a task, the second is that they are more 
cognitively complex, and the third is that text types can be arranged on developmental scale. 

Assumption 1. Concerning Value. 

Assumption: Synthesis and interpretation are, in general, more valued ways of thinking, 
more sophisticated, more typical of mature thinkers. Expert writers on tasks of this sort would, of 
course, choose to do a synthesis or Interpretation, and students should be encouraged to do 
syntheses and interpretations when they write. 

Alternative Assumption: This study led us to an alternative view of value which is a 
contextual one. The best organizing plan is the one that fits both the situation (including the 
assignment) and the writer's goals. Best" is always a trade-off of costs with benefits. 

As this picture of task representation and of critically different images of the task began to 
unfold in these classes, the students' question turned to which representation is "correct" or, to put 
it more bluntly, how can I win the lottery and pick the "right representation" ? 

Educators are likely to pose this same question in more elevated but equally evaluative 
terms~e.g.,\vhich representation is better, more intellectually scphisticated, or educationally 
valuable? Consider the following arguments we could make about the "best" organizing plan: 
The summary, we could argue, is a foundation skill in rcading-to-write. Doing a summary is 
embedded in most other processes. On the other hand, a summary by itself does not lead to critical 
literacy. A summary or review with comments can be the basis for critical thought, but it leads to 
rather limited texts. College writing calls for a more complex transformation of knowledge and 
more artful texts. 



A tex* organized as a response to the topic fosters independent thinking, but it can also be an 
archetypal avoidance strategy that eliminates the need to grapple with a source text and another 




20 

26 



person's ideas. It is often a substitute for "doing the assignment" even if the paper itself is good. 
On the other hand, I must admit that my work as a professional often depends on this plan: I begin 
to read others' work only to find it has triggered an idea of my own, and the springboard strategy 
takes over. I skim, I select, I follow my own line of thought, not the author's, and use the text 
before me to write my own. 

Synthesis may seem like a safer choice for the "best" plan to teach and encourage. Bloom's 
influential effort to rank intellectual skills has placed synthesis at the top (1956). It is clearly a 
powerful and late developing ability that is regularly invoked in academic writing. Yet, from the 
perspective of rhetoric and problem-solving, I could argue that a rhetorical image of the task is even 
more widely valuable, not only for academic work but for reading to write in life after school. A 
rhetorical, interpretive plan often embeds the acts of synthesis and summary in itself and, it could 
be argued, requires an even greater transformation of knowledge than an information-driven 
synthesis. Furthermore, this rhetorical representation of the task is even more likely to be news to 
my students, hence more worth teaching. On the other hand, a rhetorical task is a selective 
process Students who gave themselves the goal of interpreting for a purpose didn't, for example, 
see many of the contradictions in the source text They didn't necessarily engage in the same sort 
of critical thinking the synthesizers did, and so on- In Hying to answer the question, "what task 
should we do; what should we teach," there were always "other hands." 

I have sketched out this inconclusive line of argument because I think it shows that we may 
be asking the wrong question when we toy to create precise value-laden hierarchies of better or 
more lofty tasks and plans-at least in this context. The question of how to assign value sharpened 
when our group of instructors faced the issue of how to advise students, after we had helped them 
to see the power (and necessity) of their own choices. To encourage students to go for broke, to 
turn everything into a rhetorical task, for instance, or to give themselves the loftiest goals of 
creativity at every turn seemed alternately naive and hypocritical about the way writing operates in a 
context. Moreover, it simply didn't match the even more interesting reality of how active, 
professional people appear to operate. 

The reality of the task representation process seems to be much better captured by the 
metapho.* of personal costs and benefits than it does by a scale of right or wrong. Writing is a 
social, political act in the broad sense of the terms- A writer's purpose is a response to the context 
of writing. If there is little reason to reorganize or transform information, Martha's gist and list 
strategy is not only efficient but sensible. On the other hand, if a given paper assignment 
represents a step in the intellectual sequence of an entire course, it makes sense to give oneself the 
task of adapting the reading to one of the educational purposes of the course or to dealing with an 
issue that the course is raising. Taking on a task of this sort is more demanding than producing a 
summary, but the benefits are probably greater- College instructors, for instance, often expect 
students to carry out a purposeful transformation of ideas even when they don't say so directly. 

Reading to write is also a personal, intellectual act and the question of costs and benefits to 
the writer is just as critical here. Some tasks are more difficult to do, but they allow writers to go 
beyond their current understanding, 10 make something that is meaningful to themselves, or to do 
something better than they have done before. On the other hand, elevated goals that arc out of 
synchrony with time and occasion can be like the "rigid rules" Mike Rose described-inflexible 
demands which ignore that writing is "good" when it serves its purpose for the writer (1980). 
Ann, for instance, the writer's whose internal critic demanded creativity at every turn, did not feel 
she was in control of her own process or priorities. 

Conceptualizing the writer's choice in terms of costs and benefits has an economic ring that 
may seem out of place in the humanities, in the way political metaphors used to seem. However, it 
is a powerful frame for thinking about processes and decisions that often go unrecognized. 
Although we have sketched some examples of costs and benefits in this chapter, the greater value 
of the concept is probably realized in action when we encourage students to look at their own 
process. Cost/benefit is a situational concept-it points to the trade-offs that people who are 
controlling their lives always make. It also points up a central contradiction in the economics of 

21 



being a student Students do not always make die "commercial" choice-they often do not opt for 
the choice with the lowest cost and the highest short run benefit. Instead, they plunge into kinds of 
discourse they have not yet mastered, trying to talk the language, working in the faith that a " good 
idea" can pull you through and that making a serious attempt is the right thing to do. Our students 
regularly take on, try out, and plunge in when the costs in uncertainty and difficulty are high, 
because they ait willing to give priority to learning or because good teachers have made the benefits 
of trying tangible ones. My point is this: the economic metaphor of trade offs does not 
presuppose that a learner will choose the same priorities as a slum lord or low cost /high profit 
manufacturer-but it recognizes the possibility of radical differences in students' and teachers 9 
goals. In fact, it lets us examine the very differences in priorities one can set, in the benefits one 
values, and the costs and risks one is willing to incur. It also lets us recognize the common sense 
of efficiency. 

In response to Assumption 1, then, we would replace any scale which assigns intrinsic value 
to certain text types and tasks with the image of a balance, as in Figure 5. The "right" task 
representation will depend on the way a writer chooses to balance her goals, her reading of the 
situation, her priorities, the use this text has for her or others, her time, her effort, the risks, her 
relevant knowledge and so on. 



Costs Benefits 

A 

Balancing Costs and Benefits: 

Goals 
Situation 

Time 
Difficulty 
Priorities 

Figure 5. An Alternative Assumption About Value 



One implication of this view is that task representation is a social, interpretive act that may 
involve contradictions. Although "what the teacher wants" may be a legitimate question, there is no 
simple answer. Writers must "read" the situation. Student writers must learn to figure out what 
different rhetorical contexts call for and, in many cases, to infer their options from limited 
experience and inconclusive evidence. 

A second implication of this view is that the writer is empowered to make decisions about his 
or her own process and goals. In a sense the writer is unavoidably in control because reading to 
write is a purposeful process: he or she is making decisions and setting goals even when unaware 
of the options. On the other hand, this view raises the possibility of genuine empowerment in 
which writers are not only aware of options but of their own values and their decision processes. 
The question we will raise later in this study is, can we help students make this process of 
negotiating a task a more informed process? 

Assumption 2. Concerning Cognitive Complexity 

Assumption: A second assumption runs thus: Synthesis and interpretation, as we defined 
them in this study, may not always be the plans of choice, but they are the cognitively complex 
choice. They are more difficult to do, they require more intellectual maturity, and they lead to more 

22 



1 

% 

profound or complex transformations of knowledge. They are the hard tasks. 

Alternative Assumption: The alternative to this assumption would define cognitive 
complexity as a feature of the writer's process rattier than the text It says that complexity does not 
reside in an organizing scheme, a text type, or genre; complexity is a function of the goals a writer 
sets within a plan-one can write a very complex, integrative summary as well as a simple-minded 
synthesis. The complexity of a given task can be measured by the knowledge transformation 
required to do it However, the degree of knowledge transformation does not depend on a text 
type, but on the writer's prior knowledge and the extent he or she is willing or able to transform it 
Text types and genres have always been convenient pigeon holes for tracking development and 
accomplishment but they can be hazardous categories of convenience, if what we really want 
to talk about is cognitive complexity. 

Figure 6 represents this alternative view erf cognitive complexity as a continuum that goes 
from tasks that require low knowledge transformation to those that require a high degree of 
transformation-tor the individual writer on a given task . What makes such transformation 
necessary or difficult? One key variable is the writer's prior knowledge. If a student comes to 
research on "time management" with well organized background knowledge and a predetermined 
unifying concept, synthesis will not require extensive transformation of that student's knowledge, 
(cf. Langer, 1984). Another easy road to synthesis or interpretation is to bring a current idee 
fixe, a dogmatic belief, or a favorite topic to bear on whatever one reads. The cognitive task can 
thus be reduced to selecting and arranging new information in a ready-made schema. 



Example: 
Synthesis w/ 
a familiar 
idea 



T 



Low 
Transformation 



Example: 
Summary 
of complex 
discussion 



Example: 
S/nthesis or Interpretation 
that invents complex 
concept or purpose 



T 



High 
Transformption 



Knowledge Transformation Continuum Affected by: 

Prior Knowledge 
Amount and Complexity of Information 
Level of Invention 



Figure 6. An Alternative Assumption about Cognitive Complexity 



A second variable that can shoot up the demand for transformation is the amount and 
complexity of the information. For example, a graduate student with a well-defined research 
question might find it much easier to conduct an extensive literature review by doing a synthesis of 
selected "relevant" information, than by accurately summarizing the gists, key points and 
implications of thirty internally complex papers in linguistics, psychology and literary theory. Or 
imagine trying to create a well-formed summary of a particularly lively, two hour seminar 
discussion. To do that the writer might have to wrench ideas from a temporal structure, based on 
who said what to whom, to create a new thematic structure based on the key issues of the 
discussion, which he would have to infer in some cases, transforming his memory of comments 
and his sense of implied meanings and emotional enerjr *>f the speaker into gists, and finally, 
reorganizing the whole body of local topics into a meanu „ i\ unit. A final example comes from 



23 

23 



f 



ERIC 



our task in which some students chose to increase complexity by dealing with planted contraditions 
in the original text-others didnt And some students chose to use their own ideas-others didn't. 
This decision tells us something about the cognitive complexity of the task students gave 
themselves that was independent of whether they did a summary or synthesis. The complexity of 
their thinking processes was a function .of how tightly they chose to integrate how much of the 
information they possessed ; 

The third variable we observed is the demand for invention, and the level at which it occurs. 
As the examples above make clear, transforming can go on at different levels in the writer's 
hierarchy of ideas. Writers restructure their knowledge in minor ways all the time when they draw 
connecting inferences and make local transitions and in more major ways when !hey draw the 
inferences that create a sense of gist However, writing appears to make some of its most rigorous 
demands on knowledge transformation when the writer must invent information at the top level of a 
hierarchy of ideas. Some of the most extensive and most cognitively complex transformations 
come, as one would predict, when writers are attempting to forge a unique synthesizing concept 
(that can control the entire text) or when they are attempting to develop a unique rhetorical purpose 
that does justice to their goals, their knowledge, and their readers. This sot of invention is what 
gives academic writing its reputation for difficulty-even among experienced writers. 

To sum up, it would be convenient if we could measure cognitive complexity or chart 
cognitive growth by a single analysis of text features, or if we could simply equate knowledge 
transformation with certain genres and organizing plans. However, it appears that cognition can 
not be so easily reduced to the more tangible features of text As we have tried to suggest in Figure 
6, the real measure of cognitive complexity is knowledge transformation itself. Text types can 
appear at various places on that continuum. Knowledge transformation happens within the head of 
the writer-in a given situation. It is a function of prior knowledge, the amount and complexity of 
the information one is trying to transform, and the level at which invention is going on. For 
teachers, this means that conceptual difficulty is not simply a feature of the task, but a feature of the 
writer's process that depends on where the student starts and where he or she is trying to end up. 

Assumption 3. Concerning Cognitive Development 

Assumption: Our third questionable assumption is that the sequence of tasks students are 
assigned as they progress through high school and college (from summary [Applebee, 1981] to 
independent analysis and interpretation) reflects a "natural" pattern of development That is, 
students left to their own devices would progress through the modes just as our school system 
now requires them to do. The problem with this assumption of "natural" sequence is that it seems 
to go beyond the data. In recent research with kindergarteners, even the shibboleth that children's 
writing begins with narration has come under question (Dyson, 1986). Based on this presumption 
that natural development and cognitive capacities are expressed by certain modes, we soon find 
ourselves using those modes to measure capacity and in the next breath branding students who 
have not learned a given mode of discourse as cognitively or developmentally handicapped Such 
cognitive pigeonholing is especially likely when students are underprepared-just the sort of 
student who would have lacked much chance to acquire the mode we are calling "natural." 

And yet there is an important element of common sense truth observation underlying this 
assumption. I only need to consult my recent memories of student conferences or of my own 
freshman year to recognize the way students must struggle to move beyond knowledge telling, 
summary, or review and comment and come to grips with the demands for synthesis, for working 
at higher levels of abstraction, for using one's own knowledge and that of authorities, and for 
putting one's knowledge and reading to work in service of a unique rhetorical purpose. Common 
sense says that freshmen are indeed crossing a threshold that can be roughly equated with 
attempting more complex syntheses and rhetorical purposes. 

Alternative Assumption: Our alternative assumption, then, would make a much more limited 
and contextualized claim. Students come to their freshman year in college with certain forms of 
discourse under their control-typically those forms of discourse which the school system has 

24 

» 30 



asked them to practice for the last twelve years. Entering academic discourse means encountering 
new demands, learning how to meet them and practicing the same. The threshold we see students 
crossing may in reality be two different thresholds. As Figure 7 suggests, one is created by lack of 
familiarity and practice-especially for a rhetorical task. If no one has asked you to use your 
knowledge for a purpose within a community of peers, you will struggle while learning the ropes. 
The second threshold is created by way the writers of academic discourse must often plunge into 
knowledge transformaf'on-a process that is typically difficult for everyone. Students, for instance, 
often write about knowledge they are still acquiring. Scholars and researchers are expected to 
address problematic topics with unique insights and sensitive adaptation of their knowledge. 
Academic discourse values invention that occurs at the top levels of the idea structure and such 
writing is often difficult, even if one has practiced it 



Review & Synthesis Synthesis interpretation 

summary Comment (Frame) (Unique for a Purpose 

Concept) 

More Less 

Practiced Practiced 



Experience with Academic Discourse Continuum 
Figure 7. An Alternative Assumption about Development 



ERIC 



Our caution then applies to confounding development with the effects of practice or the 
inherent difficulty of knowledge transformation. If students arc being asked to represent writing 
assignments in new ways (to try for a unique, controlling concept rather than a commonplace) and 
to learn how to do these new tasks, many of the difficulties freshmen have are not a failure of high 
school education nor a problem of intellectual development Their difficulties can be a signal that 
students are in the process of learning to do a new task, which we may need to teach more 
Tplicitly than we do. 

In this discussion we have tried to lay out some of the assumptions we are making about 
how academic discourse and the tasks it poses are related to value judgments, cognitive complexity 
and practice. And we have tried to present three images of those relations that offer alternatives to 
three more familiar but problematic assumptions. We can use these three images in Figures 5, 6, 
and 7 to build a composite picture of the task many freshmen are facing in the transition from high 
school to college. Academic discourse is a game with many rules, many conventions, many 
patterns of argument and evidence. Some of the conventions are widely shared, some 
discipline-specific, but many are new to a freshman. Such discourse typically favors the text plans 
of synthesis or purposeful interpretation over the plans of summary and comment although the 
latter are the plans a freshman is most likely to control. In terms of cognitive complexity, 
academic discourse often expects not just coherence (i.e., a well-formed thesis), but the integration 
of complex material, and it (sometimes) places special value on invention at the top levels of an 
idea structure. This means that a student's prior knowledge is less likely to do the job and that the 
most demanding kind of knowledge transformation is required. Finally, because the expectations 
of a new discourse community arc by defini; on unknown, learning to represent this new 
task-even to recognize that it is in some ways new-is an important step. The benefits of trying to 
achieve a given set of goals or carry out a plan are uncertain evfn as the costs of attempting a partly 
practiced strategy go up. 

If this composite picture makes academic discourse sound difficult it may be realistic. On 

25 

* 31 



f 

the other hand, helping students see that any given assignment can be placed on the kind of 
practice and die knowledge transformation continua sketched in Figures 6 and 7 gives students 
deserved credit for what they can already do and for the new abilites they are mastering. It 
suggests that teaching has at least three agendas as well First, to move students along the 
continuum of discourse experience, we need to give students experience and practice and a more 
demystifying insight into die conventions of the discourse before them. Secondly, to encourage the 
knowledge transformation we value, we need to teach the thinking strategies that help one to invent 
and integrate. Finally, to help students make informed images of a task and its costs and benefits, 
we may need to see task representation itself as a critical part of the process we teach. 



4. TAKING METACOGNITIVE CONTROL: 
AWARENESS VS. STANDARD STRATEGIES 

This Exploratory Study raised a final issue. Is task representation a process v*hich is under 
students 1 control? Are their decisions made as a result of awareness of their options? This is a 
hard but important question to answer. 

We will begin with a brief description of some of the forms this awareness took, using the 
four part model (presented in Figure 4 )as a template to ask, what happens when writers not only 
perform an operation or possess knowledge, but are aware of their own performance or 
knowing 1 } How might awareness affect Long Term Memory, the current representation, the 
context, and the process of representing itself? When processes that can be carried out with little 
conscious thought cross the threshold into conscious attention, or when writers rise to conscious 
problem-solving, we often have an opportunity to see some of the thinking that distinguishes expert 
and novice (Flower, in press). We can also use the much more extensive research on 
metacognition in reading to help fill in the picture of how this additional level of awareness can 
affect reading to write. Although much of the reading research looks at younger readers, it helps us 
isolate those late-developing skills and demanding processes, such as comprehension monitoring, 
that probably affect adult performance too. Some problems never seem to go away. 

Awareness and Long Term Memory 

One can imagine some parts of Long Term Memory as a dimly lit storehouse of oddly filed 
and poorly cross-indexed information on all sorts of topics including summaries and syntheses. A 
great deal of information is there, but without awareness of the contents in general or a system for 
searching, it would be difficult to find and compare different pockets of knowledge. One useful 
measure of awareness, then, is the ability to conduct a metamemorial search of one's own 
knowledge, directing attention to memory itself. When Scardamalia, Bereiter and Woodruff (1980) 
asked 4th and 6th graders to generate lists of topics on which they had either high or low 
knowledge, the children found the task itself difficult; they couldn't compare their own pockets of 
knowledge in that abstract way. Moreover, the texts they wrote on high knowledge and low 
knowledge topics were indistinguishable. Knowing about your knowledge at a meta level and 
carrying out a metamemorial search appears to be a late developing skill. 

Some writers in this study clearly had some distance on and a conceptual grasp of their own 
bodies of knowledge. Like Kate, they were aware that summaries and purposeful essays posed 
meaningful options and that "relevant" topic information was a quality they defined, rather than 
found in the task. For this task we could define one critical form of awareness as: possessing 
one's own version of a menu of options, like that in Figure 2, and being able to consider and 
compare those entities a > alternatives. 

Awareness and the Current Representation 

If being able to search and reflect on oneV own stable store of knowledge is difficult, 
monitoring the changing contents of working memory in the heat of writing can be more so. 
Writers need to be aware of the changing configuration of their own image of the task. The 

26 

o 32 
ERLC 



t 



protocols show us places where writers did indeed rise to problem-solving and reflection when 
they encountered a conflict among their own goals, or between the task they had embarked upon 
and the assignment (as they read it). However, the oral presentations also registered the surprise 
writers felt at looking at the shifting sands of their own planning in these protocols and discovering 
internal conflicts that they had not recognized as problems at the time. That is, although Ann, the 
writer mentioned before, had responded to the conflict between her internal critic saying "be 
creative" and her other goal to "be comprehensive," she had not been aware of conflict itself as an 
object few: reflection. 

Awareness and Context 

Writers are inevitably in a context, which like language can be said to partially "write the 
writer" (McCormick and Waller, 1987 ). That interaction with one's context, whether it is the 
context of culture, the classroom, or the current assignment, can be automatic, reactive and 
unexamined, or it can be self-conscious and open to self-control. Critical literacy and critical 
consciousness are states of heightened awareness-knowing the covert messages context is 
sending and knowing your own assumptions and habits of response (cf, McCormick). In the 
initiation to academic discourse, Bartholomae (1985) argues, writers move from imitating and 
internalizing the conventions of a discourse to innovating within it Awareness means the ability to 
invent/infer context even when it isn't given. In one study of expert and novice readers (Haas and 
Flower, in preparation), this awareness took the form of an active strategy. When experienced 
adult readers were asked to comprehend a difficult passage out of context, they used a rhetorical 
reading strategy in which they inferred a context, purpose, and author as an aid to making the text 
meaningfully coherent This strategy was entirely missing from the performance of freshmen. 

Writers in the reading-to- write study were at times explicitly aware of the context, but they 
often chose different aspects of the assignment to promote to awareness. Here are comments from 
three students that came on the heels of re-reading the assignment 

Al. OK. Now let s see if we can put this together into a [reads] comprehensive statement 

A2. OK. [reads] The process of revision. 

A3. Topic sentence [ and writer goes on to summarize]. 

Bl. OKAY. Let's go. 
B2. Two pages she wants. 

B3. Let's get a lead [ and writer goes on to synthesize in a breezy journalistic manner]. 

C. 1 . [leads] Interpret and synthesize. 
C2. What the hell does that mean? 
C3. Synthesize means to pull together. 
C4. Not to make something up. 

C5. Why should I want to make something up? [and writer goes on to summarize]. 

Awareness is, like any cognitive process, an act of selective attention. 

While some writers were aware of the immediate audience ("Two pages she wants"), others 
saw the audience as an invitation to reflection and a constraint they had to invent In experienced 
writers awareness of audience involves going beyond recognition that readers exist to setting goals 
to affect those readers (Rower and Hayes, 1980). Here, some writers turned their sense of context 
into a self-conscious negotiation with the assignment, as we will see in detail later (cf. Peck and 
Ackerman), Awareness of this sort takes on special power because it becomes knowledge 
translated into action, as in the examples below. 



Awareness and Process 



Awareness of one's own process operates at a number of levels. The burst of recent 
research on metacognition in reading makes a distinction between the statable knowledge people 

er|c " 33 



t 

0 

have about their own thinking (including the late developing capacity to reflect on that process as it 
is happening) and the active "regulatory" knowledge that lets people guide their own process and 
employ strategies such as monitoring comprehension, planning the next move, and evaluating the 
effect of a strategy for learning (Baker and Brown, 1984). This research, reviewed by Baker and 
Brown (1984), has found that inexperienced and young readers bring surprising theories about the 
goals of comprehension to the process of reading (e.g., understanding a text means recognizing all 
the words, even if they seem unrelated). When it comes to monitoring comprehension, they fail to 
detect problems in their understanding and planted inconsistencies-^ finding that Baker has 
extended to college students as well (1979). Finally, even when readers detect breakdowns in their 
comprehension process, they may or may not be able to invoke strategies for repairing the 
problem* This research suggests that the process of monitoring and repairing gaps in 
comprehension is difficult for us all, child or adult However, the proficient college readers 
observed in process-tracing studies were distinguished by three features: their ability to talk about 
reading problems and strategies, the quantity of their comprehension monitoring comments during 
reading, and the number and kinds of strategies they used (Hare, 198 1, reviewed in Wagoner, 
1983). 

We can see parallel kinds of awareness in the writers in this study. 

a. At tbft lowest level of process awareness (and highest level of simple efficiency), we see 
writers using strategies which they invoke b y name or category (e.g., "Let's get a lead") but which 
they do not examine. 

b. At a little greater expense of metacognitive attention, writers also monitor their own 
process, noticing what they are thinking, what they have done so far, reflecting on whether it is 
working, or simply musing on their own experience. In this example, the writer is monitoring the 
associative path her own memory has taken, her own performance, and hsr current plan. 

1 . This is making me think of, one of the things I read was that inexperienced 
writers jumped into their writing and experienced writers, urn would take a little 
more time to plan. 

2. I guess if I want to fall in the category of experienced writer, I should make a 
little more of a plan myself. 

3. Urn... th<» plan that I have made so far is simply to write down either what I 
know or what 1 remember from reading this piece about revision. 

c. Awareness at the level of monitoring sometimes leads to frustration if it does no more 
than confirm that the writer is indeed in a pickle. Awareness takes on new power when the writer 
can rise to conscious problem-solving and use this awareness to actually guide the process of 
reading and writing. In studies of the ways writers conduct the processes of planning (Flower, et 
aU y in prep.) and revision (Flower, et of., 1986; Hayes, et al. y 1987; Flower, Carey and Hayes, 
1986) this ability to rise to problem solving for resolving planning conflicts and for diagnosing and 
planning solutions to problems in text was a distinctive feature of experienced writers. Under 
these circumstances the writer's goals, constraints and possible strategies become themselves the 
objects of thought as writers engage in what Bereiter and Scardamalia have called "intentional 
cognition" (1983). The writer in the example below combines a lively awareness of her own 
interests and options (her habit of "giving a purpose" to things in comment #1), with the context 
and constraints set up by the assignment Notice that in comment #2 she chooses the closing 
phrase we added for motivational effect as the instruction of interest, i.e., the purpose is to say 
what you think. In #3 she considers another potential plan (to adapt to students) and continues in 
#4 and 5 with a reflective monitoring of her own reading process, followed by an apparent 
decision to go with the initial plan (to make it interesting to myself) while the second plan (adapt 
this for my students) is left to percolate gently on a backburner. 

1. Hmmm. I kind of like to give a purpose to, to the reason why Tm writing this 
other than just to write this. 

2. [Rereads part of assignment] It just says we're interested in what you think. 

28 



34 



3. Hmm... I'm wondering if I could write this in a way that it could be used for my 
students. [Writer is a beginning instructor.] 

4. That's one of the things I was thinking about when I read this was how could I 
adapt this to be helpful to them. 

5. And one of the things that I do when I write is I have to get some overall goal or 
purpose to write. 

6. Right now Fm doing it just to make it interesting to myself. 

How Aware Are Students of Thar Choices and Process? 

The Exploratory Study showed us that individual students possessed many kinds of 
awareness about their own task representations at many levels. But as educators we want to ask, 
were most of the students operating at a level of self-awareness in all four areas proposed by our 
model? Awareness is clearly a hard quality to measure and as teachers hoping to open new doors, 
we may be pleased to overestimate the novelty of what we teach. However, the net effect of the 
protocols, the presentations, and group discussions was to suggest that metacognitive awamesss 
of the writing process and of task representation in particular is not a well-established part of the 
repertory of these students. Although they appeared clearly capable of and in possession of such 
awareness in isolated parts of their writing, they were not, appears, engaging in active 
metacognition about writing. 

Support for this tentative conclusion came from various quarters. To begin with, the sharp 
diversity in representations that emerged from the presentations was a surprise to all of us. Yet as 
the options began to take a pattern, there was a general sense that important and familiar decisions 
were being made explicit In their presentations many students registered surprise at the 
confusions, the contradictions, the inventions, and strategies that they saw in their protocols. 
Some were features of their process that they had not registered at all in the heat of composing. 
Others were strategies that they now realised were unacknowledged mainstays-and in some cases 
mainstays that could not support the more demanding work they were trying to do in college. 

In a questionnaire completed by one class in this exploratory study, 50% of the students said 
they they had not even considered the Aritten assignment closely (despite all the artfulness we had 
put into its design), but had simply invoked their "standard strategy ." That decision was clearly a 
move for efficiency, but since students' operational definitions of this all-purpose "standard" 
strategy varied so much, one wonders how good that strategy was at pleasing many and pleasing 
long, given the varied demands of college writing. Finally, what does that move say about the 
writers awareness of his or her own options, about the need to "read" the rhetorical context, and 
about the writer's control of her own cognition? The vision of a rich but unexamined process that 
emerged from this Exploratoiy Study led us to ask, what effect does task representation have on the 
reading-to- write process of freshmen-writers on the threshold of college-level academic discourse? 
This question led to the second phase of the project, the Teaching Study. The design and materials 
for the Teaching Study are presented in Report 1, "Studying Cognition in Context." The results of 
that study are analyzed from various perspectives in Reports 3-10 that make up this series entitled, 
Reading-to-W rite: Exploring a Cogni tive and Social Process. 



4 



References 

Anderson, J. R. (1980). Cognitive psychology and its implications , San Francisco: 
W. R Freen^n. 

Applebee, A. N. (1981). Writing in the secondary school (Research Monograph No. 21). 
Urbana, EL: N. C. T. E 

Arnheim, R, (1954). Visual thinking . Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. 

Ausubel, D. P. (1963). The psychology of meaningful verbal learning . New York: Grune & 
Stratton. 

Baker, L. (1979). Comprehension monitoring: Identifying and coping with text confusions. 
Journal of Reading Behavior, 11(4), 365-374. 

Baker, L., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Metacognitive skills in reading. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamill, & 
P. Mosenthal. Handbook of read : g research . New York: Longman. 

Bartholomae, D. (1985). Inventing the university. In M. Rose (Ed.), When a writer can't write: 
Studies in writer's block and other composing-process problems (pp. 
134-165). New York: The Guilford Press. 

Bazerman, C. (1981). The informed writer . Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 

Bereiter, C, & Scardamalia, M. (1983). Schooling and the growth of intentional cognition: 
Helping children take charge of their own minds. In Z. Lamm (Ed.), New 
trends in education (pp. 73-100). Tel-Aviv: Yachdev United Publishing Co. 

Bereiter, C, & Scardamalia, M. (1982). From conversation to composition: The role of 
instruction in a developmental process. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in 
instructional psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 1-64). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence 
Erlbaum Associates. 

Bizzell, P., & Herzberg, B. (1986). Review of "What makes writing good." College 
Composition and Communication , 37(May), 244-247. 

Bloom, B. S. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives: Cognitive and affective domains 
(Vols. 1-2). New York: David McKay. 

Britton, J. et al. (1975). The development of writing abilities (1 1-18) . London: Macmillan. 

Brown, A. L., & Day, J. D. (1983). Macrorules for summarizing texts: The development of 
expertise. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 22, 1-14. 

Dyson, A. H. (1986). Transitions and tensions: Interrelations between the drawing, talking, and 
dictating of young children. Research in the Teaching of English, 20(Dec.), 
379-409. 

Flower, L. (in press). Taking thought: The role of conscious processing in the making of 
meaning. In E. Maimon, B. Nodine, & F. O'Connor (Eds.), Thinking, 
reasoning, and writing . New York: Longman. 

Flower, L., Carey, L., & Hayes, J. (1986). Diagnosis in revision: The experts' option 
(Communications Design Center Tech. Rep.). Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie 
Mellon University. 

Flower, L., & Hayes, J. (1980). The cognition of discovery: Defining a rhetorical problem. 
College Composition and Communication, M, 21-32. 

Flower, L., & Hayes, J. (1984). Images, plans, and prose: The representation of meaning in 



ERLC 



36 



writing. Written Communication, 1(1). 120-160. 

Flower, L., Hayes, J., Schriver, K., Carey, L., & Haas, C. (in prep.). Planning in writing: A 
theory of the cognitive process . Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Mellon University. 

Flower, L., Hayes, J. R., Carey, L, Schriver, K., & Stratman, J. (1986). Detection, diagnosis, 
and the strategies of revision. College Composition and Communication, 37, 
16-55. 

Fieedman, A., & Pringle, I. (1980). Writing in the college years: Some indices of growth. 
Gojggg Composition and Communication, 31 (Oct), 311-324. 

Glick, J. (1975). Cognitive development in cross-cultural perspective. In F. D. Horowitz (Ed.), 
Review of child development research (Vol. 4). Chicago: University of 
Chicago Press. 

Goodnow, J. J. (1976). The nature of intelligent behavior: Questions raised by cross-cultural 
studies. In L. B. Resnick (Ed), The nature of intelligence (pp. 168-188). 
University of Pittsburgh: Lawrence Erlbaum. 

Haas, C, & Flower, L. (in prep.). Rhetorical reading strategies and the construction of meaning. 

Hare, V. C. (1931). Readers' problem identification and problem- solving strategies for high- 
and low-knowledge comprehenders. Journal of Reading Behavior, 13, 
359-365. 

Hayes, J., & Flower, L. (1981). Writing as problem solving. Visible Language, 14, 388-399. 

Hayes, J., Flower, L., Schriver, K., Stratman, J., & Carey, L. (1987). Cognitive processes in 
revision. In S. Rosenberg (Ed.), Advances in applied psycholinguistics: 
Reading, writing, and language processing . Cambridge, England: 
Cambridge University Press. 

Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city . Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. 

Langer, J. A. (1984). Effects of topic knowledge on the quality and coherence of informational 
writing. In A. Applebee (Ed), Contexts for learning to write . Norwood, NJ: 
Ablex. 

McCormick, K. (1985). Theory in the reader Bleich, Holland and beyond. College English, 
47(Dec.), 836-850. 

W >rmick, K., & Waller, G., with Flower, L. (1987). Reading texts: Reading, responding, 
writing . Washington, DC: Heath. 

Meyer, B. (1982). Reading research and the composition teacher: The importance of plans. 
College Composition and Communication, 33(Feb.), 37-49. 

Penrose A. et al. (in preparation). Studying cognitive processes in the classroom . Pittsburgh, 
PA: Center for the Study of Writing, Carnegie Mellon University. 

Piaget, J. (1932). The language and thougH of the child (M. Gabin, Trans.). New York: 
Harcourt, Brace and Co. 

Rose, M. (1980). Rigid rules, ir^exible plans, and the stifling of language: A cognitivist 
analysis of writer's block. College Composition and Communication, 31, 
389-401. 

Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1987). Knowledge telling and knowledge transforming in 
written composition. In S. Rosenberg (Ed), Advances in applied linguistics . 
New York: Cambridge University Press. 

3 7 



Shaughnessy, M. (1977). Some needed research in writin ,. College Composition and 
Communication. 28(Dec.), 317-321. 

Simon, H. A. (1973). The structure of ill-structured problems. Artific ial Intelligence 4 
181-201. 6 

Spivey, N. N. (1984). Discourse synthesis: Constructing texts in reading and writing. Newark, 
DE: International Reading Association. 

Thorcau, H. D. (1964). In C. Bode (Ed), T he portable Thorcau . New York: Viking Press. 

Voss, J. F., Greene, T. R, Post, T. A., & Penner, B. C. (1983). Problem solving skills in the 
social sciences. In G. Bower (Ed), The psychology of learning and 
motivation: Advances in Research and Theory (Vol. 17). New York: 
Academic Press. 

Wagoner, S. A. (1983). Comprehension monitoring: What it is and what we know about it 
Reading Research Quarterly. 18(3), 328-346. 



38 



APPENDIX 1 

The following instructions wercptrt of the practice session which trained students to collect their 
own thinking-aloud protocols. They were designed to help clan, y the dual roles of the writer and 
the protocol collector, although the use of a second person was optional. 



Thinking Aloud While You Write 
Instructions for the Writer 

« 

In the process of writing, people think and say many things to themselves that are quickly 
forgotten. Yet these thoughts are interesting and important parts of the writer's problem-solving 
process. 

We are interested in the thoughts that go through your head as you work on this problem. 
We are asking you to do 3 things: 

1. Work on the task as you normally would: read, think, jot notes, or just write. 
(However, don't erase. Simply cross through anything you don't intend to use.) 

2. While you are reading, thinking to yourself, or writing-please read and think aloud, 

even as you are writing something down. 

3. We are NOT asking you to talk ahnut your process, or to explain or justify what you are 
doing. We want you to focus all your attention on doing the task. Simply think out loud, as if 
you were talking to yourself as you solved the problem. 

********************************************************** 
Instructions for the Protocol Collector 

1. Find a place that will be free from interruptions. (Put a sign on the door. ) Have paper, 
pens, tape and everything ready before the writer gets the assignment (Coffee & tea are fine; no 
gum.) 

2. Before the writer begins, prepare the tape recorder by reading the subjects name, the 
date, and the name of the task onto the tape. Then test the tape by playing your introduction 
back. Put the mike on a towel or quiet surface and test for good volume and placement of the 
mike. Cue the tape to^tart at the end of your intro. 

3. Ask the writer to do a short warm up session (3-4 minutes) thinking aloud on a practice 
task then listening for where you had to prompt before trying again. During the practice session 
prompt the writer by simply saying w What are 3 ou thinking now?" whenever the 
writer falls silent for more than 3*5 seconds. 

4. Make sure the writer knows when the first side of the tape will be full and can turn the 
tape over if you will not be there. 

5. TURN THE TAPE ON. Stay with the writer for the first part of the session to prompt the 
writer whenever he or she falls silent If the writer mumbles, turn up the recorder and ask the 
writer to SPEAK UP. 

6. When the session is over, collect all the materials-notes, drafts, text Make sure 
the writer's full name, phone and address are written on the text Make sure notes and pages are 
NUMBERED in the order in which they were written. You may need the writer's help to get this 
figured out 

7. Finally, protocols are typed double spaced, no paragraphs, with name and date, and 
with dots (. . .) for short pauses and underlined spaces ( ) for unintelligible fragments. 

ERIC 3 ^ 



APPENDIX 2 



Reading and Interpreting Data 

Here is a short passage, including research results and observations, on the performance of 
experienced writers. Your task is to read and interpret this data in order to make a brief (1-2 page), 
comprehensive statement about the pmoai fif revision in writing. Your statement should intcoret 
and synthesize all of the relevant findings in the text As you read, please read out loud, and whca 
anything crosses your mind, say out loud whatever you art thinking, even if it seems irrelevant or 
incomplete. Do whatever you would normally do, except say aloud whatever you are noticing or 
thinking to yourself as you read and make your statement IVople think and do many different 
things while they are reading: we are interested in how you do it 



THE PASSAGE 

Some Recent Findings on Writing 

Recent research has found a number of differences between die writing processes of good 
writers and weak writers. When Pianko (1979), for example, timed the various actions of college 
student writers, she found that students enrolled in the remedial writing course (die weak writers) 
began writing about 40 seconds after they were given a topic. Students enrolled in the standard 
freshman writing course, on the uiher hand, waited for ovef a minute before they began to 'write. 
Pianko assume i that the students were using the time before they began writing to plan their 
essays, and concluded that the stronger writers did more planning than did the weaker writers. 

Writers who approach writing as a problem-solving activity tend to treat editing and 
revising as useful steps in composing because these activities break the process up, making it easier 
to handle. They find that many of the problems that block writing can be solved when they return 
to the work as editors, and that editing is an inexpensive method (in terms of time and effort) for 
making dramatic improvements in writing. These writers fed that editing lets then concentrate on 
communicating with a reader. 

Sommers (1980) interviewed a number of people who said they were experienced writers 
and like to write. She found that when they wrote, they normally did more than one draft and that 
they talked about revision as if it were re-vision, that is, a chance to resee their whole paper and 
possibly rethink and reorganize the whole thing. The students she interviewed described revision 
as cutting and "slashing out" unnecessary works, weak parts and errors. They usually didn't 
revise their papers. 

Many textbook writers say that effective writers are re writers. A first version, they often 
feel, is never as good as a second version, a second one as good as a third, and so on, salfing & 
the changes from version to version are made for good reasons . But how do good writers arrive at 
good reasons? They evaluate their writing in three ways: 

1 . They set standards or criteria by which it can be judged 

2. They relate their subject to the criteria. 

3. They draw the conclusions fot follow. 

Halpern and Liggett (1984) studied the writing of a number of business people who 
regularly dictated their letters, memos and reports. These people did a good deal of planning, but 
they did very little revision, especially of the spoken "draft" These researchers also found that 
when textbook writers and other researchers talk about the writing process, they are often thinking 
of the writing process of a certain, limited group of writers-essayists, journalists, academic, and 
creative writers, but not business people. 

40 



Good writers review their goals for their papers and the plans they used to implement these 
goals. Then they study their papers with those high-level goals in mind. They test to see not only 
if the papers fit their goals and plans, but also to see if their plans changed in midstream. Faigley 
and Witte (1981) found that when the experienced writers and journalists they studied revised a 
text, they made changes that affected the meaning of the text, not just the wording. Even when 
they changed only single words those changes would have altered a summary of the piece. The 
inexperienced student writers in their study stuck to finding errors and altering individual words, 
but their changes did not alter the meaning. 

Revising often produces shorter words and shorter sentences* Witte (1983) asked a large 
group of students to read and revise a lon$ paragraph ftom a textbook. After judges had sorted the 
revisions into "good" and "poor" ones, Witte found that everyone had made the paragraph shorter 
and used simpler words. However, the successful revisers had made deletions that emphasized the 
"gist," meaning, or main point of the original text; they had connected and subordinated the other 
sentences to it The unsuccessful revisers did not seem to use the gist of the original to organize 
their revision, and their paragraphs had no single, clear focus. 

TASK: Now go ahead and write down (on another piece of paper, please) your statement 
about the process of revision in writing based on your interpretation of this data. 

Please think out loud as you do this. Be sure to let the tape recorder know if you are doing 
any rereading and what you are looking at Make any notes, marics, or changes you want to, but 
please do not erase anything if you change your mind; just cross things out And try to say 
everything that crosses your mind, eve.* fragments and stray thoughts. 

Thank you. We are interested in what you think. 



41